About this Journal Submit a Manuscript Table of Contents
Autism Research and Treatment
Volume 2012 (2012), Article ID 146132, 8 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/146132
Review Article

The Development of Executive Function in Autism

1Centre for Research in Autism and Education (CRAE), Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, London WC1H 0AA, UK
2School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia

Received 9 February 2012; Accepted 7 May 2012

Academic Editor: Hilde M. Geurts

Copyright © 2012 Elizabeth Pellicano. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Autism is a common and often highly debilitating neurodevelopmental condition, whose core behavioral features are believed to be rooted in disrupted neurocognitive processes, including especially “executive function.” Researchers have predominantly focused upon understanding the putative causal relationship between difficulties in EF and autistic symptomatology. This paper suggests, however, that the effects of individual differences in EF should be more far-reaching, playing a significant part in the real-life outcomes of individuals with autism, including their social competence, everyday adaptive behavior, and academic achievement. It further considers the nature of the EF-outcome relationship, including the possible determinants of individual differences in EF, and makes several recommendations for future research.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum conditions (hereafter, “autism”) are a set of common, lifelong neurodevelopmental conditions that involve substantial heterogeneity at numerous levels, including etiology, neurobiology, cognition, and especially behavior. Long-term follow-up studies show that the developmental outcomes of autistic individuals are highly variable, even for individuals at the more intellectually able end of the autism spectrum. While some individuals go on to live independently and obtain qualifications, the majority fail to achieve independence, to attain full-time employment, or to enjoy friendships [15] (though see [6]). Explaining this variability is of critical import: to discover why developments take place in some areas and not in others, and especially in some individuals and not in others.

Researchers have little understanding of the factors underpinning this heterogeneity, due in part to a dearth of longitudinal studies tracing the development of autism and to the preponderance of studies using a case-control design, focusing on group rather than on individual differences. This paper aims to address this limitation. It identifies one potential source of this variability, namely, autistic children’s emerging “executive function” (EF), those higher-order processes, closely associated with the prefrontal cortex, which are necessary for regulating and controlling behavior (see Table 1). Specifically, it suggests that individual differences in the growth trajectories of autistic children’s EF skills could explain some of the variability in children’s functional outcomes, including their social awareness, real-life adaptive behavior, and readiness to learn in school, both in the shorter term and in the longer term.

tab1
Table 1: Definition of key constructs and examples of tasks used to test these skills in preschool and primary school-age children.

2. Executive Functions in Autism

EF has received extensive attention in the autism literature for many years largely due to the influential proposal that the inherent rigidity and invariance of autistic behaviors could be explained by a primary impairment in executive control [7, 8]. EF problems have been demonstrated consistently in school-age children, adolescents, and adults with autism [8, 9], as well as relatives of individuals with autism albeit to a lesser degree [10, 11] (see [12, 13] for reviews). These problems typically manifest as perseverative responses (i.e., getting “stuck” performing the same action) and difficulties switching flexibly between response sets. Furthermore, problems in specific components of EF have been shown to distinguish autism from other developmental conditions, such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [14, 15] (though see [16]).

Despite the theory’s apparent face validity, several negative findings have cast doubt over the possibility that a primary problem in EF can explain all autistic symptoms. Not all individuals show EF difficulties (e.g., [1719]), and investigations of EF in young children with autism have failed to find evidence of autism-specific difficulties [20, 21]. Consequently, researchers have shifted away from a framework that emphasizes a single, primary neurocognitive atypicality (such as EF) as the underlying cause of autism to one that encompasses multiple cognitive atypicalities [22, 23].

3. Why Focus on Autistic Children’s Developing Executive Function?

While there is general consensus that EF problems are unlikely to play a primary causal role in autism, it remains possible that the degree of difficulties in EF could play a substantial role in autistic children’s developmental outcomes—including their social competence (those skills, including “theory of mind,” which allow individuals to evaluate social situations and respond effectively; see Table 1), their adaptive behavior (those skills necessary for individuals to live independently and to function well in real-life settings), and their success in school. Indeed, clinicians and those who care for individuals with autism often associate some individuals’ inability to achieve independence with persistent difficulties in regulating behavior and adapting flexibly to change (e.g., [2]). Therefore, whether poor EF plays a fundamental role in the emergence of core autistic features or, instead, is a consequence of early atypical input from another cognitive system, it is nevertheless likely to place the child with autism at risk for a poor developmental outcome either directly or indirectly.

Substantial recent progress in understanding the typical development of EF provides further reason to investigate individual differences in EF as a possible source of the heterogeneity in outcomes in autism. First, it is clear that EF is intimately tied to another aspect of neurocognitive development known to develop atypically in autism: theory of mind (ToM: mental state awareness). Numerous studies report robust correlations between individual differences in tasks tapping ToM and EF, independent of the effects of age and IQ, in typical preschoolers [2427] and toddlers [28, 29]. Several theorists have further proposed a direct, functional link between EF and ToM. They assert that the abilities to monitor one’s actions and to act with volition are critical for reflecting on the mental states of self and other, that is, that ToM emerges from EF [30, 31]. Research findings have repeatedly shown that early EF skills are predictive of later ToM but early ToM skills are not predictive of later EF [2729], providing overwhelming support for the view that EF is critical to developmental changes in ToM.

Second, early EF is also predictive of typical preschoolers’ later school readiness [32, 33] and academic success in reading and mathematics [3436]. The transition to school relies on mastery of basic EF skills, including remembering and following instructions, completing tasks independently and smoothly transitioning between tasks, and inhibiting inappropriate behaviors. EF, therefore, is held to play an important role in the acquisition of knowledge; the better children are at focusing and refocusing their attention, holding information in mind and manipulating it and resisting distraction, the better placed children should be to acquire knowledge and skills in the classroom.

Third, it is well known that the prefrontal cortex, which mediates EF, shows a protracted development trajectory: it begins to develop very early in life, has a boost during the preschool period, and continues to develop well into adolescence [37]. The extended postnatal developmental of prefrontal cortical networks means that they should be particularly sensitive to exogenous influences. Several studies now report direct evidence of the malleability of EF: “exercising” young children’s EF skills results in substantial improvements in their ability to regulate their behavior in both at-risk [38] and typical [39] populations.

Taken together, these findings provide compelling grounds for suggesting that one source of the heterogeneity in autistic individual’s functional outcomes are individual differences in emerging EF. Some researchers have begun to speak to this possibility, with several studies reporting links between EF and a range of concurrent behaviors, including social competence (e.g., [18, 40]) and everyday adaptive behavior (e.g., [41, 42]). Yet full consideration of this developmental issue has not yet been possible due to a paucity of studies analyzing individual differences within a longitudinal design. The handful of studies using such an approach have done so, though, with dramatic success. Griffith et al. [20] found links between early executive skills (performance on a spatial reversal task tapping cognitive flexibility) and sociocommunicative behavior (bids for joint attention) one year later in young children with autism, but not in nonautistic children with developmental delay. Also, individual differences in set-shifting ability predicted the social competence scores of cognitively able adults with autism 3 years later in one study [43] and, in another, predicted real-life adaptive skills between 11 and 27 years later [5].

A recent study has further demonstrated the influence of autistic children’s early EF skills on their sociocognitive awareness. Pellicano [18] investigated the EF and ToM skills of 37 cognitively able children with autism and 31 typical children (M age = 5 yrs 6 mths). As expected, children with autism showed difficulties in both domains compared to typical children. There were also significant EF-ToM correlations, suggestive of a functional link between domains. Analysis of patterns of “atypicalities,” however, revealed striking dissociations in one direction only: poor ToM coupled with intact EF [18]. In line with Russell [31], these findings suggested that EF skills might be one important ingredient for the development of ToM but that ToM does not play this role for the development of EF.

Three-year follow-up data on the same samples of children further supported this claim [54]. For children with autism, individual differences in early EF skills (Time 1) were longitudinally predictive of developmental change in ToM skills (Time 2), independent of age, language, nonverbal intelligence, and early ToM skills. Yet there were no predictive relations in the opposite direction—a finding that is entirely consistent with longitudinal studies of the EF-ToM relation in typical children [2729]. These results provide initial evidence that autistic children’s cognitive skills emerge within a dynamic developing system where EF skills play a critical role in shaping the developmental trajectories of ToM.

In sum, accumulating evidence of the important contribution of EF in typical development, together with promising findings from studies with individuals with autism, provides good reason to suspect that individual differences in the development of EF might critically influence autistic children’s developmental trajectories and could account, at least in part, for the heterogeneity in their sociocognitive, behavioural and academic success.

4. How Is EF Organized in Autism and Which Factors Drive Its Growth?

Determining the precise nature of the developmental course of EF in autism and also of the potential causal links between EF and other social and learning outcomes demands greater understanding of the nature of EF itself and of the mechanisms underpinning its growth. Although there is no question that EF plays a vital role in well-regulated, organized behavior [55], there has been much disagreement regarding the characterization of the EF construct. Like adult models [56, 57], competing developmental accounts differ with respect to which they emphasize the unitary [58, 59] or fractionated [60, 61] nature of EF. Empirical work with adults, using sophisticated statistical techniques like confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), has reported evidence of three latent EF variables or component processes—set-shifting, updating (working memory), and inhibitory control—which are partially independent but still intercorrelated [62].

Studies using similar methods with school-age children in part support this integrated framework [63, 64], although recent studies with 2- to 6-year-old children have instead reported evidence in line with a unitary model of EF [65, 66]. These latter results question the apparent continuity in the structure of EF during development but are consistent with a dynamic, neuroconstructivist approach in which cognitive functions begin relatively undifferentiated and become progressively modularised or specialised over time [67] (see Figure 1). This framework suggests that individual differences in development itself might be key to explaining the wide variation in findings both within [68] and across [12, 16] studies on EF in autism.

146132.fig.001
Figure 1: A simplified model showing a unitary executive function (EF) construct early in development and an emergent fractionated construct of EF with development with latent EF variables of working memory (WM), inhibitory control (IC), and set-shifting (SS).

No study has yet explicitly investigated the nature of EF in autism. Early fractionation of EF makes it plausible for distinct EF components, such as cognitive flexibility, to be specifically affected in autism. Yet if, as the evidence suggests, EF is a single, unitary construct during early childhood (at least in typical children), then it becomes more difficult to see how a distinct profile of EF difficulties might emerge in autism. Given the prolonged development of EF and the degree of neural plasticity during childhood [69, 70], it is likely that emerging prefrontal cortical networks affect, and are affected by, the development of other key cognitive functions. In this case, then, disruption to distinct EF components in autism might be driven by other factors.

One goal therefore should be to understand precisely which mechanism(s) drives the development of EF in typical children and in children with autism. Some theorists propose that progress in EF occurs via the development of the prefrontal cortex [60] and the strengthening of prefrontal representations [58] in an experience-dependent manner [71]. An influential yet contrasting view suggests that development in children’s attentional control—the ability to focus on a task and ignore irrelevant information—is the source of common variance in EF [61, 72]. Posner et al. have demonstrated significant advances during the preschool period in the central “attention network,” which includes alerting, orienting, and executive attention processes [73, 74]. Developmental changes in attention are considered to provide children with greater executive control over action. On this view, then, rate of growth in EF should be predicted by developments in attentional capacities (Figure 2(a)) and such developments might even mediate the fractionation of EF.

fig2
Figure 2: Three simplified models showing executive function (EF) development. Model (a) includes a latent construct of attention that influences baseline levels (EF intercept) and rate of growth of EF (EF slope). Model (b) shows a latent construct of language mediating baseline EF and its developmental trajectory. Model (c) includes latent constructs of attention and language, which both influence the development of EF. These models could be tested using latent growth curve modelling.

Impairments in core attentional processes have been reported in autism, including problems with disengagement or so-called “sticky” attention [7577]. Fundamental problems in critical aspects of attention could therefore place limits on the rate of EF development, which could in turn hinder the emergence of autistic children’s social and learning outcomes. It is of course possible that the causal relationship might exist in the opposite direction, such that early developments in EF might influence the emergence of attentional networks. The relationship between aspects of attention and components of executive control has, however, been hitherto unaddressed in autism.

Yet Posner and Rothbart’s [72] model (see also [61]) neglects the potentially mediating role of another key function: language. Children’s verbal skills can affect the expression of EF, for example, by limiting their ability to store phonological information in working memory [78]. Yet language might play a more fundamental role, affecting the very development of EF. For several theorists [31, 7981], language provides an internal plan for behavior. Vygotsky [81] emphasized the importance of self-directed speech during early childhood, which becomes increasingly internalized during the preschool years, allowing children (verbally) to “think through” problems and to guide future-oriented behavior. Similarly, Zelazo et al. [59] stress that language is the medium through which higher-order (if-if-then) rules are formulated and is key to recursive thought. Developmental gains in young children’s language skills (specifically their ability to formulate hierarchical rules) are therefore directly implicated in the rate of EF development (Figure 2(b)).

Difficulties with communication are a core characteristic of autism [82] and has been previously implicated as a potential limiting factor on the development of EF (e.g., [83]). Furthermore, children with autism are less likely to use verbal rehearsal strategies on executive tasks [8486] suggesting that they, unlike typical children, may not be using internal language in the service of executive control. Pellicano’s [54] longitudinal work showed that autistic children’s initial receptive-vocabulary skills were not predictive of EF performance three years later, suggesting further that verbal skills may not influence the emergence of EF in autism as they do in typical development [26, 28]. Individual differences in the growth trajectories of autistic children’s verbal skills therefore might partially mediate (or fail to mediate at all) the rate of progress of EF in autism. Further still, it is possible that attention and language could mediate the development of EF. In this case, both functions could make independent contributions to the rate of growth of EF (Figure 2(c)) and both could be limiting factors in autism. Importantly, evidence for any one of these patterns (Figures 2(a)2(c)) would suggest that the potential influence of EF on children’s functional outcomes is indirect rather than direct.

All of these models suggest that the development of EF itself might be shaped by certain endogenous factors, which in turn could mediate children’s developmental outcomes. Yet the developmental trajectory of EF, and its resulting neurocognitive architecture, will be an emergent property of interactions within the children and between the children and their environment. Alternative explanations therefore place the development of EF squarely in the social realm. For example, Luria emphasized that “we must go beyond the limits of the individual organism and examine how volitional processes are formed for the child in his/her concrete contacts with adults” [87, page 89] (see also [88]). Hughes [27] (see also [80]) extended this view to suggest that the effect of EF upon the development of ToM should be indirectly influenced by the child’s social environment. Since negotiating social interactions requires children to regulate their own behaviors (e.g., turn-taking, following rules in games), peer relations are likely to have positive effects on children’s developing executive skills, which in turn will foster their developing ToM.

There has been renewed interest in the sociocultural predictors of EF development, which so far include socioeconomic status [89], parent scaffolding [90, 91], and parent-child interactions [92]. These exogenous factors are also likely to influence the development of EF in children with autism. Contrary to popular opinion, children with autism do not grow up in a social vacuum. Rather, they can show attachment security with caregivers [93], can engage in positive and collaborative interactions with siblings (e.g., [94]), and actively seek out their nonautistic peers [4]. It is therefore plausible that social contact could influence autistic children’s developing EF, which in turn might exert its effects on key real-life outcomes.

5. Future Perspectives

There is much awareness of the huge variability in autistic children’s developmental outcomes but there is very little empirical knowledge of the underlying causes of this variability. Early individual differences in EF represent one candidate source of this heterogeneity. To address this issue, however, we need a richer understanding of the causal determinants of EF growth in autism, which will require prospective longitudinal studies and carefully designed training studies. Well-designed cognitive training studies will need to disentangle whether any gains are genuinely “raising the ceiling” of performance or just “speeding up development” to reach the child’s given ability. Such knowledge will lead ultimately to a more nuanced theoretical, and distinctly developmental, perspective of EF in autism. Elucidating whether EF has direct or indirect longitudinal effects on children’s functional outcomes is vital for knowing whether to directly “exercise” autistic children’s EF skills or to concentrate instead on bootstrapping those factors (attention/language/social environment) through which EF influences children’s outcomes.

References

  1. P. Howlin, S. Goode, J. Hutton, and M. Rutter, “Adult outcome for children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 212–229, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  2. P. Howlin, L. Mawhood, and M. Rutter, “Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 41, no. 5, pp. 561–578, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  3. L. Kanner, Childhood Psychosis: Initial Studies and New Insights, Winston/Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 1973.
  4. M. Sigman and E. Ruskin, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial no. 256, vol. 4, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Ill, USA, 1999, Social competence in children with autism, Down syndrome and other developmental delays: a longitudinal study.
  5. P. Szatmari, G. Bartolucci, R. Bremner, S. Bond, and S. Rich, “A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 213–225, 1989. View at Scopus
  6. M. A. Farley, W. M. McMahon, E. Fombonne et al., “Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities,” Autism Research, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 109–118, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  7. C. Hughes and J. Russell, “Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: its implications for theories of autism,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 498–510, 1993. View at Scopus
  8. S. Ozonoff, B. F. Pennington, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 32, no. 7, pp. 1081–1105, 1991. View at Scopus
  9. C. Hughes, J. Russell, and T. W. Robbins, “Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 477–492, 1994. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  10. C. Hughes, M. Leboyer, and M. Bouvard, “Executive function in parents of children with autism,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 209–220, 1997. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  11. C. Hughes, M. H. Plumet, and M. Leboyer, “Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 40, no. 5, pp. 705–718, 1999. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  12. E. L. Hill, “Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism,” Developmental Review, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 189–233, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  13. N. Russo, T. Flanagan, G. Iarocci, D. Berringer, P. D. Zelazo, and J. A. Burack, “Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 77–86, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  14. H. M. Geurts, S. Verté, J. Oosterlaan, H. Roeyers, and J. A. Sergeant, “How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and autism?” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 836–854, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  15. S. Ozonoff and J. Jensen, “Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 171–177, 1999. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  16. H. M. Geurts, B. Corbett, and M. Solomon, “The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 74–82, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  17. M. Liss, D. Fein, D. Allen et al., “Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 261–270, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  18. E. Pellicano, “Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: clues to developmental primacy,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 974–990, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  19. E. Pellicano, “The development of core cognitive skills in Autism: a 3-year prospective study,” Child Development, vol. 81, no. 5, pp. 1400–1416, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  20. E. M. Griffith, B. F. Pennington, E. A. Wehner, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive functions in young children with autism,” Child Development, vol. 70, no. 4, pp. 817–832, 1999. View at Scopus
  21. B. E. Yerys, S. L. Hepburn, B. F. Pennington, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive function in preschoolers with autism: evidence consistent with a secondary deficit,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 1068–1079, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  22. G. Dawson, J. Munson, A. Estes et al., “Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay,” Child Development, vol. 73, no. 2, pp. 345–358, 2002. View at Scopus
  23. F. Happé, A. Ronald, and R. Plomin, “Time to give up on a single explanation for autism,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 10, pp. 1218–1220, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  24. S. M. Carlson and L. J. Moses, “Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind,” Child Development, vol. 72, no. 4, pp. 1032–1053, 2001. View at Scopus
  25. D. Frye, P. D. Zelazo, and T. Palfai, “Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning,” Cognitive Development, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 483–527, 1995. View at Scopus
  26. C. Hughes, “Executive function in preschoolers: links with theory of mind and verbal ability,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 233–253, 1998. View at Scopus
  27. C. Hughes, “Finding your marbles: does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind?” Developmental Psychology, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 1326–1339, 1998. View at Scopus
  28. S. M. Carlson, D. J. Mandell, and L. Williams, “Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 40, no. 6, pp. 1105–1122, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  29. C. Hughes and R. Ensor, “Executive function and theory of mind: predictive relations from ages 2 to 4,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 6, pp. 1447–1459, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  30. L. J. Moses, “Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development,” Child Development, vol. 72, no. 3, pp. 688–690, 2001. View at Scopus
  31. J. Russell, “How executive disorders can bring about an adequate theory of mind,” in Autism as an Executive Disorder, J. Russell, Ed., pp. 256–304, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 1997.
  32. C. Blair, “School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry,” American Psychologist, vol. 57, no. 2, pp. 111–127, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  33. M. M. McClelland, C. E. Cameron, C. M. Connor, C. L. Farris, A. M. Jewkes, and F. J. Morrison, “Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary and maths skills,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 947–959, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  34. C. Blair and R. P. Razza, “Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten,” Child Development, vol. 78, no. 2, pp. 647–663, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  35. R. Bull and G. Scerif, “Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 273–293, 2001. View at Scopus
  36. H. L. St Clair-Thompson and S. E. Gathercole, “Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 745–759, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  37. A. Diamond, “Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, D. Stuss and R. Knight, Eds., pp. 466–503, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2002.
  38. A. Diamond, W. S. Barnett, J. Thomas, and S. Munro, “The early years: preschool program improves cognitive control,” Science, vol. 318, no. 5855, pp. 1387–1388, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  39. J. Holmes, S. E. Gathercole, and D. L. Dunning, “Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children,” Developmental Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. F9–F15, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  40. G. Dawson, A. N. Meltzoff, J. Osterling, and J. Rinaldi, “Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism,” Child Development, vol. 69, no. 5, pp. 1276–1285, 1998. View at Scopus
  41. L. Gilotty, L. Kenworthy, D. O. Black, A. E. Wagner, and L. Sirian, “Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders,” Child Neuropsychology, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 241–248, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  42. F. Happé, R. Booth, R. Charlton, and C. Hughes, “Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 25–39, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  43. H. J. C. Berger, F. H. T. M. Aerts, K. P. M. Van Spaendonck, A. R. Cools, and J. P. Teunisse, “Central coherence and cognitive shifting in relation to social improvement in high-functioning young adults with autism,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 502–511, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  44. D. Wechsler, Wechsler Scales of Intelligence, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 4th edition, 2003.
  45. B. Milner, “Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man,” British Medical Bulletin, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 272–277, 1971. View at Scopus
  46. C. Hughes, “Control of action and thought: normal development and dysfunction in autism: a research note,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 229–236, 1996. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  47. A. R. Luria, K. H. Pribram, and E. D. Homskaya, “An experimental analysis of the behavioral disturbance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal endothelioma (meningioma),” Neuropsychologia, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 257–280, 1964. View at Scopus
  48. P. D. Zelazo, “The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children,” Nature Protocols, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 297–301, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  49. T. Shallice, “Specific impairments of planning,” Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, vol. 298, no. 1089, pp. 199–209, 1982. View at Scopus
  50. S. Baron-Cohen, A. M. Leslie, and U. Frith, “Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ?” Cognition, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 37–46, 1985. View at Scopus
  51. H. Wimmer and J. Perner, “Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception,” Cognition, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 103–128, 1983. View at Scopus
  52. P. Mundy, A. Hogan, and P. Doehring, A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social-Communication Scales, University of Miami, Coral Gables, Fla, USA, 1996, http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/ourteam/faculty_staff/escs.pdf.
  53. S. Sparrow, D. Cicchetti, and D. Balla, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Pearson Assessment, Minneapolis, Minn, USA, 2nd edition, 2005.
  54. E. Pellicano, “Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 530–544, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  55. J. Duncan, “An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 11, pp. 820–829, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  56. J. Duncan and A. M. Owen, “Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 23, no. 10, pp. 475–483, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  57. T. Shallice and P. W. Burgess, “Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man,” Brain, vol. 114, no. 2, pp. 727–741, 1991. View at Scopus
  58. Y. Munakata, “Graded representations in behavioral dissociations,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 5, no. 7, pp. 309–315, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  59. P. D. Zelazo, U. Müller, and D. Frye, “The development of executive function in early childhood,” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 1–27, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  60. A. Diamond, “The early development of executive functions,” in Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change, E. Bialystock and F. I. M. Craik, Eds., pp. 70–95, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2006.
  61. N. Garon, S. E. Bryson, and I. M. Smith, “Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 31–60, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  62. A. Miyake, N. P. Friedman, M. J. Emerson, A. H. Witzki, A. Howerter, and T. D. Wager, “The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 49–100, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  63. M. Huizinga, C. V. Dolan, and M. W. van der Molen, “Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 44, no. 11, pp. 2017–2036, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  64. J. E. Lehto, P. Juujärvi, L. Kooistra, and L. Pulkkinen, “Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 59–80, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  65. S. A. Wiebe, K. A. Espy, and D. Charak, “Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: i. Latent structure,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 575–587, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  66. S. A. Wiebe, T. Sheffield, J. M. Nelson, C. A. C. Clark, N. Chevalier, and K. A. Espy, “The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 436–452, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  67. A. Karmiloff-Smith, “Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 56–63, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  68. K. J. Towgood, J. D. I. Meuwese, S. J. Gilbert, M. S. Turner, and P. W. Burgess, “Advantages of the multiple case series approach to the study of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 47, no. 13, pp. 2981–2988, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  69. P. R. Huttenlocher, Neural Plasticity: The Effects of the Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2002.
  70. C. A. Nelson, K. M. Thomas, and M. de Haan, Neuroscience and Cognitive Development: The Role of Experience and the Developing Brain, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006.
  71. W. T. Greenough, J. E. Black, and C. S. Wallace, “Experience and brain development,” Child Development, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 539–559, 1987. View at Scopus
  72. M. I. Posner and M. K. Rothbart, “Developing mechanisms of self-regulation,” Development and Psychopathology, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 427–441, 2000. View at Scopus
  73. M. R. Rueda, J. Fan, B. D. McCandliss et al., “Development of attentional networks in childhood,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 42, no. 8, pp. 1029–1040, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  74. M. R. Rueda, M. I. Posner, and M. K. Rothbart, “The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 573–594, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  75. J. A. Burack, “Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 103, no. 3, pp. 535–543, 1994. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  76. R. Landry and S. E. Bryson, “Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with austism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 6, pp. 1115–1122, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  77. T. A. Mann and P. Walker, “Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 274–284, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  78. A. Baddeley, Working Memory, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1986.
  79. A. R. Luria, The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior, Pergamon Press, London, UK, 1961.
  80. A. R. Luria, Higher Cortical Functions in Man, Basic Books, New York, NY, USA, 1966.
  81. L. Vygotsky, Thought and Language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.), MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1962.
  82. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR), American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, 2000.
  83. J. Russell, Agency: Its Role in Mental Development, Erlbaum, Hove, UK, 1996.
  84. R. M. Joseph, S. D. Steele, E. Meyer, and H. Tager-Flusberg, “Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory?” Neuropsychologia, vol. 43, no. 10, pp. 1400–1411, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  85. G. L. Wallace, J. A. Silvers, A. Martin, and L. E. Kenworthy, “Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in Autism spectrum disorders,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 39, no. 12, pp. 1735–1739, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  86. A. J. O. Whitehouse, M. T. Maybery, and K. Durkin, “Inner speech impairments in autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 47, no. 8, pp. 857–865, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  87. A. R. Luria, Language and Cognition, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1981.
  88. L. Vygotsky, Mind and Society: The Development of Higher Mental Processes, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1978.
  89. K. G. Noble, M. F. Norman, and M. J. Farah, “Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children,” Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 74–87, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  90. A. Bernier, S. M. Carlson, and N. Whipple, “From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning,” Child Development, vol. 81, no. 1, pp. 326–339, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  91. C. H. Hughes and R. A. Ensor, “How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function?” New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, vol. 2009, no. 123, pp. 35–50, 2009. View at Scopus
  92. B. L. Rhoades, M. T. Greenberg, S. T. Lanza, and C. Blair, “Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 638–662, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  93. A. H. Rutgers, M. J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. H. van IJzendoorn, and I. A. van Berckelaer-Onnes, “Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic review,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 6, pp. 1123–1134, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  94. F. Knott, C. Lewis, and T. Williams, “Sibling interaction of children with autism: development over 12 months,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 10, pp. 1987–1995, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus