Abstract

To investigate the effects of psychological stress on the masticatory muscles of rats, a communication box was applied to induce the psychological stress (PS) in rats. The successful establishment of psychological stimulation was confirmed by elevated serum levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) and changed behaviors in the elevated plusmaze apparatus. The energy metabolism of the bilateral masseter muscles was tested via chemocolorimetric analysis, whereas muscle ultrastructure was assessed by electron microscopy. In comparison to the control group, the PS group showed evidence of swollen mitochondria with cristae loss and reduced matrix density in the masticatory muscles after three weeks of stimulation; after five weeks of stimulation, severe vacuolar changes to the mitochondria were observed. Increased vascular permeability of the masticatory muscle capillaries was found in the five-week PS rats. In addition, there was decreased activity of - ATPase and -ATPase and a simultaneous increase in the activity of lactate dehydrogenase and lactic acid in the masticatory muscles of PS rats. Together, these results indicate that psychological stress induces alterations in the ultrastructure and energy metabolism of masticatory muscles in rats.

1. Introduction

A number of studies have examined the role of psychological stress in the development of masticatory muscle disorders (MMD). War-related psychological stress, the performance of mental arithmetic, and the solving of five-letter anagrams can increase masticatory muscle activity and are believed to be associated with MMD [13]. Moreover, psychological tension can induce muscle fatigue and spasm, resulting in MMD [46]. A relationship between stress and MMD has been reported in children, adolescents, and adults [79]. Additionally, most patients with MMD experience moderate to severe depression or anxiety [10, 11]. On the other hand, cognitive-behavioral [12], habit reversal [13], and biobehavioral treatment [14] strategies are all known to be beneficial in the management of MMD, thus reflecting another line of evidence demonstrating a relationship between psychological stress and MMD.

All of the studies conducted to date illustrate the importance of psychological factors in MMD [15, 16]. However, it is not yet possible to determine how the mental/psychological factors affect the muscle and even to facilitate the MMD. Clinical studies using traditional electromyography (EMG) to assess the role of psychological stress in MMD have shown significantly increased EMG activity of masticatory muscles under stress conditions [3, 1719]. Similar results have been observed in animals [20, 21]. Under the emotional stress, changes in the masticatory behaviour and hormones such as glucocorticoids were accompanied by a modification of particular MyHC isoforms of the muscle, which showed evident faster phenotype due to a higher percentage of either pure MyHC-2b containing fibres or hybrid MyHC-2x/d and -2b expressing fibres [22]. Furthermore, a decrease in oxygen saturation of muscle blood around the onset of mental stress proved the potential role for psychological stress in hemodynamic alterations, which might occur via their regulation by the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) [23, 24]. An elevation of histidine decarboxylase activity in skeletal muscles during stress was induced, partly or wholly by muscle activity and/or muscle tension [25]. However, muscular mechanism underlying emotional stress is not well understood and appears to be equally complex.

The hypothesis of this paper was that psychological stress could alter the ultrastructure and energy metabolism of masticatory muscles. Using an emotional stress paradigm, which employed the use of intraspecies emotional communication within a communication box [8], we introduced experimental correlates of psychological stress, such as anxiety and depression, in rats and then tested our hypothesis on their masseter muscles. The masseter muscle is useful in this type of research because there are large amounts of spindles in these muscles [26], which are critical in determining muscle contraction and mandibular movement in response to stimulation [27]. This study sought to assess alterations in the ultrastructure and energy metabolism of masticatory muscles of rats under psychological stress.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Experimental Design

Forty-eight male Wistar albino rats weighing between 160 and 180 g (approx. 35 days old) were housed in 80 cm 45 cm 40 cm cages in a temperature-controlled room at C under a 12-hour light/dark cycle and were given free access to food and water. Before experiments, they were housed in a communication box one hour a day for five days to allow them to acclimatize to the box. The rats were then randomly divided into control (CON), foot-shocked (FS), and psychological stress (PS) groups, with each group having 16 rats. FS and PS rats were housed in one communication box during the procedure as described below, with the FS rats receiving electric foot shock and the PS rats being subjected to psychological stress. The rats in the control group were housed in another communication box under the same conditions but were not subjected to electric foot shock or psychological stress. The same study was performed twice, and the durations of psychological stimulation were three weeks and five weeks. Thus, 24 animals were used per study, with eight animals in each of the three groups.

After three or five weeks of stimulation, the behaviors of the eight rats from the control or PS group were immediately evaluated by an elevated plus-maze apparatus. Blood was drawn from the ophthalmic artery, and serum samples were prepared for the measurement of serum stress indices. The rats were then sacrificed by intraperitoneal injection of an overdose of thiamylal sodium, and their bilateral deep masticatory muscles were removed. All muscle samples were obtained according to guidelines established by the University Internal Review Board for use of Mice Subjects. The experimental procedures were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Fourth Military Medical University. The middle bellies of each muscle sample were dissected for ultrastructure observation by electron microscopy.

2.2. Animal Model for Psychological Stress

The communication box (16) was selected as the psychological stress apparatus in this study. It consisted of 16 compartments that were 16 16 cm each and were separated by transparent plastic boards with several small holes. The boards prevented the animals from making physical contact with each other but allowed them to receive cues, such as visual, auditory, and olfactory sensations, from the neighboring animals. Each compartment was equipped with a grid floor of stainless steel rods, which were 5 mm in diameter and placed at intervals of 0.3 cm. A 48-V electric generator, which was made by the Department of Biomedical Engineering of the Fourth Military Medical University, was connected to the grid floor to produce an electric current and generate an electric foot shock every two seconds. The grid floors of eight of the compartments were covered by plastic plates to prevent electric foot shock and served as non-foot-shock compartments for the PS rats (Figure 1).

Stress stimulation within the communication box commenced five days after electrode installation. Prior to the day of stress stimulation, the PS rats, together with the FS rats, were individually confined in each compartment of the communication box for one hour per day without any electric foot shock for one week to allow them to acclimatize to the surroundings. The electric foot shock was then introduced to the FS rats (stress senders) for one hour a day at a fixed time in the morning. The PS rats (stress responders) confined in the non-foot-shock compartments were simultaneously exposed to emotional cues from the neighboring FS rats, such as shrieks, smells of urine or feces, and jumping responses. Consequently, the PS rats were assumed to be in a state of fear or anxiety. The purpose of this study was to explore the effects of the pure psychological stress upon the masseter muscles. But the foot-shocked group rats experienced a great part of physical stress, so we did not include this group in the experimental evaluations. The foot-shocked group was only used to induce psychological stress in the neighboring PS rats, and they were not included in the following investigations.

2.3. Behavioral Evaluation

The elevated plus-maze (EPM) apparatus (RD1208, Shanghai Mobiledatum Corporation, Shanghai, China) [28] consisted of two open arms (50 10 cm) and two enclosed arms (50 10 cm, with 50-cm high walls) extending from a central square platform (10 10 cm). The same types of arm were arranged in the opposite position. The apparatus was elevated 50 cm above the floor in a quiet room with temperature controlled at C, with the light just bright enough to clearly observe the rats movement within a 1.5-meter range. Two of the opposing arms (50 cm 10 cm) were enclosed by 40-cm high side and end walls (enclosed arms), the other two arms were not installed with walls (open arms). In the beginning, animals were placed in the central area (10 10 cm) of the maze, facing an enclosed arm. The exposure lasted for five minutes, and certain parameters were recorded. Entry into one arm was recorded when an animal placed all four paws past the line dividing the central square from the open arms. The test arena was wiped with a damp cloth after each trial. The number of entries into the open/closed arms and the time spent in open arms/closed arms were measured by an observer blind to the treatment conditions of the animals. Then, the percent of open arm entries (100 open/open enclosed entries, OE%) and time spent in the open arms (100 open/open enclosed arm time, OT%) of the EPM were calculated for each rat. We used OE% and OT% to analyze the anxiety level of each animal [2932].

2.4. Serum Assay

The serum concentrations of cortisol and adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), stress indices, were measured by radioimmunological analysis kits (Northern Bioengineering Institute, Beijing, China) according to the protocols provided by the manufacturer.

2.5. Ultrastructure

The middle bellies of the bilateral deep masseter muscles were dissected and cut into 1-mm pieces and then fixed with 4% glutaraldehyde and 1% osmic acid. The samples were embedded in Epon812, sectioned by LKB-V ultramicrotome (LKB, Bromma, Sweden), and stained with uranyl acetate and lead citrate. The ultrastructures of the muscles were then observed with transmission electron microscopy (JEM-100SX, JEOL Company, Japan).

2.6. Energy Metabolism

A sample of masticatory muscle (0.2 g) was minced and homogenized with 1.8 mL distilled water. Then, 0.1 mL homogenates were diluted 10-fold with water. After incubation for 15 min at room temperature, the mixtures were then centrifuged for 10 min at 3000 r/min. The supernatants were transferred into new tubes and stored at C before further examination. The activities of Na+-K+ATPase, Ca2+-ATPase and lactate dehydrogenase (LDH) and the contents of lactic acid (LD) were measured by commercial kits according to the protocols provided by the manufacturer (A070-5, A019-2, A020, Nanjing Jiancheng Bioengineering Institute, Nanjing, China) using a UV-754 spectrophotometer (Shanghai Third Analytical Instrument Factory, Shanghai, China). The protein concentrations were measured via the Bradford method, with BSA as the standard [33]. The substrate for ATPases is ATP, which is split by the ATPase into ADP and inorganic phosphorus (Pi). Determination of the amount of Pi indicates the level of ATPase activity. Units of ATPase activity were shown as the content of Pi generated per hour (h) by decomposition of protein per gram (g), and the total protein content was determined by Lowry method. Therefore, the amount of inorganic phosphorus was used to detect the activity of Na+-K+ATPase and Ca2+-ATPase at the wavelength of 636 nm. With regard to LDH, it catalyzes the reaction that generates pyruvic acid from lactic acid; pyruvic acid can then react with 2,4-dinitrophenylhydrazine to produce pyruvate dinitrophenylhydrazone, which is brownish red in alkaline solutions. Enzyme activity can be calculated through colorimetry; the detecting wavelength is 440 nm. Further, by catalytic dehydrogenation of LDH, lactic acid changes into pyruvate and NAD+ changes into NADH. By dehydrogenation of PMS, NBT deoxygenizes purple-colored objects; the relationship between this change and LD is linear and occurs at an absorbance of 530 nm. Therefore, the wavelength for Na+-K+, Ca2+-ATPase detection LDH detection, and LD detection was 636 nm, 440 nm, and 530 nm, respectively. The enzyme activities were expressed as the incorporation of total phosphate into the substrate per mg protein per hour. In this part, we stored the supernatants for no more than 6 hours at C and finished the detection of all samples in one day.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

Experimental data were analyzed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) across the control group, the three-week PS group, and the five-week PS group using SPSS, version 11.0 (SPSS Co., Chicago, Illinois). The SNK-q test was further used to calculate any differences between the two groups. A P value less than  .05 was considered statistically significant.

3. Results

To confirm that the experimental rats were in a state of stress, we first analyzed the serum levels of cortisol and ACTH in the rats. As shown in Figure 2(a), the concentrations of cortisol were 12.46 2.67 ng/ml and 11.78 2.35 ng/ml in the PS group after three or five weeks of psychological stimulation, respectively. These values were markedly increased compared with those of the control group (6.19 1.47 ng/ml and 5.93 1.54 ng/ml, resp., P   .05). Similar changes were observed in ACTH levels. As shown in Figure 2(b), in the PS group, the serum ACTH concentrations were 25.46 5.12 ng/ml and 23.49 4.95 ng/ml in rats after three and five weeks of stimulation. By contrast, during the same periods, the serum concentrations of ACTH in the control group were only 12.22 2.56 ng/ml and 12.54 2.79 ng/ml, which were significantly lower than those in the PS group (P   .05), indicating the rats in PS group were anxious.

We then compared the behaviors of rats after three or five weeks of psychological stimulation with those of the control group by EPM tests. As shown in Figure 3(a), percentages (%) of entry into open arms (open arm entry, OE) in the PS rats were 34.02 2.89% and 24.66 1.99% after three or five weeks of stimulation, respectively, while those of control group were 48.94 2.94% and 48.65 3.81%, respectively, which were significantly higher than the PS groups. Similarly, as shown in Figure 3(b), percentages of time spent in open arms (OT%) significantly decreased from 70.39 3.03% in the control group to 46.14 4.05% in the PS group after three weeks of psychological stimulation and from 72.25 1.35% in the control group to 25.05 3.07% in PS group after five weeks (P   .05), indicating that the rats in PS group were mentally under tension.

Next, we examined the ultrastructures of the masticatory muscles from both the control and PS groups after three and five weeks of psychological stimulation. As shown in Figure 4, the ultrastructures of the masticatory muscle cells of rats in the control and PS groups showed evenly distributed muscular nuclei under the sarcolemma around muscle fibers and no signs of hyperplasia, swelling, or pyknosis (Panels 1, 4, and 7). Myofibril and myotome of the PS rats were as normal as the rats in the control group. The light I bands and dark A bands of myotomes in the PS rats were intact and in the right position (Panels 4 and 7). Swollen mitochondria with cristae loss and reduction of matrix density were found in the 62.5% (5 of eight samples) and 87.5% (7 of eight samples) of the masticatory muscles samples of 3-w and 5-w PS rats, respectively, (as marked by black arrows in Panel 5 and 8). These ultrastructural observations are consistent with the possibility that the mitochondria from muscles of stressed mice may have altered permeability characteristics. Moreover, dramatic vacuolar changes in mitochondria appeared in PS rats after five weeks of stimulation (as marked by white arrows in Panel 8), which approximately were found in all samples (87.5%, 7 of eight samples). In contrast, there were nearly no swollen/abnormal mitochondria or vacuolar changes being observed in unstressed animal, which also gave the proof that the mitochondria changes observed in the PS rats has no relationship with the TEM sample processing.

The ultrastructure of capillaries in all control specimens were normal. The cytoplasm contained ribosomes, small strands of rough endoplasmic reticulum, and occasional small mitochondria. The outer membrane of each capillary was generally smooth, whereas their inner surfaces were often more convoluted with small cytoplasmic processes projecting into the lumen (Panel 3). The outer capillary wall was enclosed within a basal lamina approximately 20 nm in thickness. Portions of pericytes were often attached to the outer aspect of capillary basal laminae. The morphology of most capillaries in each specimen of 3-w PS rat was essentially normal, and the endothelial cell thickness appeared to be similar to that of the control specimens. But 2 of eight samples showed a sign of electron density decrease in the capillary cells (as marked by black arrow in Panel 6). As to the capillaries ultrastructure of the 5-w PS rats, cytoplasm of some capillary cells appeared to be more electron-lucent than those in control specimens (as marked by black arrows in Panel 9), with an increase in vesicle number and membrane ruffling. Some places of the capillary basal laminae seemed accidented or even discontinuous (as marked by white arrows in Panel 9), which appeared in about 75% of all samples (6 of eight samples) and meant local hypoxia and increased vascular permeability to some extent. These results implied that the ultrastructures of masticatory muscle cells were distinctly affected in PS rats after three and five weeks of psychological stimulation.

With regard to energy metabolism in the masticatory muscles, Na+-K+ATPase activity in the masticatory muscles decreased significantly from 7.60 0.50  mol Pi/mg protein/hour in the control group to 4.10 0.53  mol Pi/mg protein/hour in the PS rats after three weeks of stimulation and even decreased from 7.15 0.35  mol Pi/mg protein/hour in the control group to 2.50 0.32  mol Pi/mg protein/ hour in the PS group after five weeks of stimulation (P   .05) (Figure 5(a)). Similarly, Ca2+-ATPase activity of the masticatory muscles decreased from 8.38 0.23 and 8.47 0.37 Pi/mg protein/hour in the control group to 3.82 0.58  mol Pi/mg protein/hour (P   .05) and to 2.14 0.43  mol Pi/mg protein/hour (P   .05) in the PS rats after three and five weeks of stimulation, respectively, (Figure 5(b)). By contrast, the activities of LDH and the contents of LD, respectively, significantly increased from 32.41 2.35 (U/g protein) and 0.67 0.10 mmol/g protein in the control group to 85.13 9.63 (U/g protein) and 1.25 0.19 mmol/g protein in the PS group after three weeks of stimulation (P   .05) and even increased to 128.69 10.19 (U/g protein) and 1.97 0.33  mmol/g protein in the PS group after five weeks of stimulation (P   .05) (Figures 5(c) and 5(d)). There were also significant differences in the activities of Na+-K+ ATPase, Ca2+-ATPase, and LDH and in the contents of LD between the three-week and five-week PS groups (P   .05).

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to report significant changes in the ultrastructures and energy metabolism of the masticatory muscles of rats exposed to psychological stress.

The communication box is a well-established method for introducing psychological stress to animals. In this paradigm, the animals that do not undergo physical stress are able to perceive the responses of their neighboring animals that are exposed to physical stress, which is delivered through an electric foot shock. The intraspecies emotional communication then signals the nonstimulated rats to become anxious and can cause increased plasma level of stress hormones, such as corticosterone, in the experimental subjects [34]. In this paper, we successfully simulated a psychologically stressful environment using the communication box as indicated by the increased levels of cortisol and ACTH (Figure 2) and the decreased OT% and OE% in the EPM test (Figure 3) after three and five weeks of psychological stimulations.

It has been demonstrated that psychological stress is accompanied by regional modification of muscle morphology, possibly due to differential gene expressions or accessibility to hormones in certain regions of the masticatory muscle [22]. The present study observed subtle but significant changes in the ultrastructure of the rat masticatory muscles under psychological stress. Transmission electron microscopy demonstrated swollen mitochondria with cristae loss and reduction of matrix density in rats under psychological stress for three weeks (Figure 4, Panels 2, 5, and 8) and severe vacuolar changes in the mitochondria of the rats under a longer period of psychological stress (Figure 4, Panels 3, 7, and 9), suggesting that there were likely some important changes in mitochondrial function secondary to psychological stress. Therefore, we further investigated energy metabolism in the muscle, which is primarily associated with mitochondrial function.

Na+-K+ATPase and Ca2+-ATPase are key factors that help to maintain and modulate mitochondrial function. Increased ATPase activity accelerates the rate of metabolism of muscle cells and improves muscle exercise capacity. Decreased ATPase activity is one of the signs of cell damage, which is often associated with hypoxia, acidic metabolites, or free radical formation [35]. Increases in lactate, concomitant with glycogen breakdown, are the result of an increased need by the muscle for an anaerobic energy supply. LDH activity level plays an important role in the removal of lactic acid and maintaining normal pH in the tissues, and it is an important enzyme in anaerobic oxidation [36]. We found that psychological stress results in decreased Na+-K+ATPase and Ca2+-ATPase activity, increased LDH activity, and elevated LD content in masticatory muscles, indicating a decrease in aerobic glycolysis and hypoxia or ischemia of the muscles. With increased exposure to psychological stress, the aerobic metabolism of the masticatory muscle decreased (Figures 5(a) and 5(b)), while anaerobic metabolism increased (Figures 5(c) and 5(d)), implying correlations between mitochondrial ultrastructure and energy metabolism in rat masticatory muscles. Similar results have been observed in the atrial cardiomyocytes of rats exposed to audiogenic stress for 6 h [37].

Psychological stress, such as anxiety and tension, has been reported to increase myoelectricity [3, 18] and excessive activities of the masticatory muscle, including bruxism [21]. It has been suggested that the hemodynamics of jaw muscles are susceptible to mental stress via their regulation by the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) [23, 24], which is generally beneficial in the acute state of stress as it helps the organism to cope with changing environmental conditions and to reobtain homeostasis. Hidaka et al. observed a decrease in oxygen saturation of muscle blood around the onset of mental stress [23]. This is consistent with our observations by energy metabolism analysis showing decrease in aerobic glycolysis and hypoxia or ischemia of the muscles by mental stress. Widegren et al. reported that the vascular response of skeletal muscle in individuals under mental stress is mainly a vasodilation, rather than a vasoconstriction [38], which is consistent with the widened space around the capillaries and increased vascular permeability observed in this paper by TEM.

Emotional stressors induce masseter muscle contractions [3941] that may be based on a pathway and may, in part, be mediated as follows: sensory inputs occur via the thalamus (activation of the amygdale, especially basolateral complex). Efferents to various CNS systems, included muscular contraction, via ventral amygdalofugal pathway to the brainstem, specifically the trigeminal motor neurons [42]. The effect on cranial nerve nuclei and components induced by emotional stress (cranial nerve V, masseters; cranial nerve VII, muscles of facial expression) also lead to mouth opening and jaw movements that are supposedly facial expressions of fear [16]. There also has been theorized that compression of the blood vessels caused by muscle contraction could result in the release of local myogenic and metabolic vasodilatation factors. Therefore, adequate blood flow is usually guaranteed in the working muscles, as the sympathetic vasoconstriction is antagonized and overridden by the powerful local vasodilator action of metabolites released by the contracting muscles. The imbalance between these actions, the excessive muscle contraction/metabolites, and an insufficient vasodilation induced by the SNS may create ischemia, which might be responsible for the decreased aerobic metabolism and increased vascular permeability of masseter capillaries.

Clinically, these results emphasize the importance of environmental influences on the emotional state, and these influences could in turn affect the ultrastructure and energy metabolism of masticatory muscles. Whether these alterations are reversible or reducible needs to be further investigated with countermeasures, such as drugs, physiotherapy, and psychological stress-free environments.

5. Conclusions

Psychological stress can cause mitochondrial injuries and hyperemia of the masticatory muscle capillaries in rats. Further, such stress can result in dramatic alterations in the energy metabolism of the masticatory muscles. Following prolonged psychological stress, these changes become more severe. Thus, this might reflect a possible mechanism for the pathogenesis of masticatory muscle dysfunction.

Acknowledgments

This study was financially supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (no. 30672326). The authors are deeply thankful to Professor Jian Liu of the Fourth Military Medical University of China (Xi’an, China) for his earnest guidance. Y. J. Chen and F. Huang contributed equally to this work.