Abstract

Aim. The quantitative genetics underlying correlated behavioural traits (‘‘animal personality’’) have hitherto been studied mainly in domesticated animals. Here we report the repeatability ( ) and heritability ( ) of behavioural types in the highly social cichlid fish Neolamprologus pulcher. Methods. We tested 1779 individuals repeatedly and calculated the of behavioural types by variance components estimation (GLMM REML), using 1327 offspring from 162 broods from 74 pairs. Results. Repeatability of behavioural types was significant and considerable (0.546), but declined from 0.83 between tests conducted on the same day, to 0.19 on tests conducted up to 1201 days apart. All estimates were significant but low (e.g., pair identity SE). Additionally, we found significant variation between broods nested within the parent(s), but these were not related to several environmental factors tested. Conclusions. We conclude that despite a considerable , in this cichlid species is low, and variability in behavioural type appears to be strongly affected by other (non)genetic effects.

1. Introduction

Individuals within animal populations often differ consistently in how they cope with environmental and social challenges, for instance with some individuals typically reacting shy and nonaggressively to such (novel) challenges and others reacting bold and aggressively [13]. Often, these individual differences are referred to as “behavioural types” or “coping styles” (e.g., shy, bold), and behavioural traits may covary amongst each other (e.g., shy individuals are also nonaggressive and nonexplorative, whereas bold individuals are more aggressive and explorative, e.g., [4]). Correlated behavioural trait values on the population level are commonly denoted as animal personalities, behavioural syndromes, or temperaments [5]. Central questions in animal personality research are (i) whether differences in behavioural types have a genetic basis, that is, whether they are heritable; (ii) whether and to what extent individuals remain consistent in their behavioural traits over time, that is, whether behavioural responses of individuals are repeatable [68].

The genetic components affecting the expression of different personalities have been well explored in humans (see reviews e.g., [915]), domesticated animals [16, 17], and animal model systems [1820]. The evolutionary ecological factors responsible for the evolution of variation in behavioural types remains somewhat enigmatic and difficult to explain in natural animal populations, despite recent theoretical advances showing how life-history tradeoffs might generate and maintain such variation [21, 22]. In principle, consistent individual differences and covariation in behavioural traits are a paradox in evolutionary biology, particularly if such differences have a genetic basis. Standard theory would expect each individual to flexibly adjust their behaviour to the environment. For instance, in a predator rich environment each individual should devalue future fitness in favour of current fitness and adjust their behaviour accordingly (e.g., hide more and be less explorative).

In the human psychology literature it has been acknowledged that although personality differences have a genetic basis [915], other (social) factors might impinge upon and alter the expression of human behavioural types over a lifetime, including for instance life events (like the death of a partner, [23]). If personalities are truly fixed over life and can be accurately measured by using standardized questionnaires (e.g., [24]), or by using standardized observational assessments (e.g., [2528]), or behavioural tests (as used to determine personalities in many invertebrates and vertebrates, see [2]), the repeatability of behavioural types should approach one. Clearly, this is not the case in humans ([29]; where it increases from 0.31 in childhood to a plateau of 0.73 beyond 50 years of age: [30]) and in most animal studies [7], which led to the proposition to incorporate behavioural reaction norms into the animal personality concept [6]. By incorporating behavioural reaction norms, animal behaviour can be analysed using standard game theory, where behavioural strategies may be governed by internal “state” (e.g., body condition, sex, status, and reproductive activity), sometimes resulting in alternative strategies (e.g., depending on morphology or age), but where these strategies and behaviours are not necessarily fixed for life (i.e., without the need to assume the existence of animal personalities). In fact, this has been the major criticism of animal personality research: if the long-term stability (“repeatability”, ) is not proven in any study population, why not assume that the current variability between individuals reflects the current variability in “states” of the individuals tested? Using a meta-analysis, Bell and others showed that the repeatability estimate significantly declined with the time interval between the different tests [7]. As the number of long-term repeatability studies in animal personality research is still very low [31] and biased towards domesticated animals [3242], our first target is to test for the long-term repeatability of behavioural types, using descendants of a wild population of a cooperatively breeding cichlid, Neolamprologus pulcher.

We use the cichlid N. pulcher as model species, where individuals have been shown to differ consistently in behavioural traits (across the bold-shy continuum) in both the field [43] and the laboratory (stocks derived from the same field population, [4, 39, 4446]). In addition, males and females have been shown to remain relatively stable in trait values from the juvenile stage (when they are the small, subordinate helpers of an adult pair) to early adulthood (when they are large, subordinate helpers and reproductively mature [39]). Behavioural types in this species may also influence sociality, reproduction [46], and helping behaviour [39, 44, 47]; and also alloparental brood care differs consistently between female helpers [47]. The major hypothesis proposed to explain this variability in behavioural types suggests that subordinates trade-off effort to gain social dominance inside versus outside their territory, which either selects for distinct life-history strategies (e.g., nonexplorative, helpful, and risk-aversive individuals opt for dominance inside their group’s territory, whereas explorative, bold, and aggressive individuals opt for dominance outside their group’s territory, which involves early dispersal and independent breeding [39, 48]) or for diverse ontogenetic trajectories in behavioural types (e.g., young fish being risk aversive whereas older fish being risk prone). Here we expand the time frame of standardised tests to encompass the lifetime of these fish.

The second target of this study has been to estimate the genetic variance underlying phenotypic variation in personality traits in N. pulcher (“heritability” [49, 50]), and to compare estimates of and . Repeatability often sets the upper limit to the heritability of a trait, and both measures are correlated in comparisons across species or populations, if the phenotypic variances in the compared units are governed by similar processes involving additive genetic variance. Any species or population showing a high heritability in a behavioural trait should also show a high repeatability in this trait (as genes are more likely to be involved in the determination of the behaviour). In contrast, a high repeatability can coincide with a low heritability, if the behavioural type is based, for instance, on the (current) internal state of individuals or on their (life-history) strategy [21] or social strategy [51], which may cause variation that is largely independent from genetic effects. Maternal or paternal effects, maternal additive genetic effects and genotype × environment interactions may also yield discrepancies between the heritability and the repeatability estimates (e.g., with heritability exceeding the repeatability, [52]). Both low repeatability and low heritability would indicate that the population exhibits no animal personalities, particularly if the repeatability diminishes over time.

In Neolamprologus pulcher, personality traits (such as boldness, aggressiveness, and propensity to explore) are consistently different between individuals (see references above) and related to two major life-history decisions: whether to help and whether to disperse. Furthermore, these traits are not related to growth rate in fish kept singly [39], but if living in groups consisting of members with divergent personality types, shy fish were found to grow quicker in body length than bold fish [53]. Studies of personalities are particularly interesting in social species like cichlids and primates [54], as they may bear similarities to the human personality axes which incorporate significant aspects of human behaviour and sociality (e.g., “OCEAN”: Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, but with the possible exception of Neuroticism [24, 5558]). Human and animal personality differences may be governed by similar differences in (neuro)physiological responses to environmental challenges and stressful situations [23, 5962]. In this study we first estimated the repeatability of behavioural types based on a combined score of boldness, aggressiveness, and exploration propensity by comparing test results of individuals obtained successively with intervals ranging from 0 to 1201 days (which approaches the maximum life span of this species: see [63]). In a second step we estimated the heritability of these same behavioural types using parent(s)-offspring regressions and variance component estimations from offspring derived from different broods of the same pair.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Species

Descendants of wild caught Neolamprologus pulcher (from wild animals collected in 1996, 2006, and 2009 near Kasalakawe, Zambia, so-called three different “stocks” and their crossings) were used for this study and tested in the years 2005–2008 and 2010. These fish are well-studied cooperative breeders [64], endemic to Lake Tanganyika where they live in breeding groups composed usually of a dominant breeder male, one to several breeder females and some helpers [65]. All fish were fed twice a week with fresh food (JBL Cyclops spp., shrimp, Artemia spp., mosquito larvae) and the other five days with JBL De Novo Lake Tanganyika cichlid flake food (except for some missing days due to absence). This is the standard feeding regime for all cichlid fish at our laboratory. During 2010 we additionally fed all individuals on six to seven days per week with fresh small food items (Artemia freshly hatched eggs and JBL Cyclops spp., the latter replaced with Daphnia spp. when all offspring of all broods had grown beyond 10 mm standard length). This was done to ensure that all offspring received enough food to grow proficiently and to reach testing age. Cichlids were kept in tanks within climatised rooms (24–29°C, lights on between 08:00–21:00 h).

2.2. Experimental Setup

The experimental setup and tests performed in 2005 and 2006 were reported in [39] (repeated tests of the same 36 individuals up to 150 days apart by Roger Schürch), tests performed in 2007 were reported in [4] (repeated tests of the same 272 individuals up to 53 days apart by Susan Rothenberger). In 2008 (unpublished) repeated tests were performed of 32 individuals by Liana Lasut, Estée Bochud, and Sebastian Keller (using the same setup as [4]), and 10 individuals retested in 2010 by Noémie Chervet and Dik Heg). These data were used to calculate the repeatability of behavioural types for time intervals of up to 1201 days after the first test was done. Individuals were marked between 2005 and 2008 with colour marks or fin clips and kept in breeding pairs, or fitted with PIT transponders and kept in aggregation tanks up to the moment of re-testing in 2010.

In September 2009 new breeding pairs were established and allowed to breed without monitoring their clutches (all “control pairs”), their offspring were tested without any information about the clutches (and thus offspring were up to 6 months of age). Between March and August 2010 these pairs were augmented with more new breeding pairs that were allowed to breed as follows.

Control pairs ( pairs) were kept in 89 or 93 litre tanks (length × breadth × height cm, height water level cm: 60 × 40 × 40 cm, 37 or 50 × 50 × 40 cm, 37 cm; resp.) and one clutch was left to hatch inside their tank (usually the first clutch), and these offspring remained there until personality testing (Figure 1(a), clutch treatment “with parents”, see below for more details). All other clutches were removed on the day of egg laying and put separately to hatch in a 24-litre tank (40 × 25 × 25 cm, 24 cm; clutch treatment “isolation”, see below for details). As pairs may produce a clutch about every other week [66], removal of the clutches ensured that we knew the identity of the offspring if they remained with their parents. However, some clutches remained undetected and were discovered after hatching and in such cases we removed all the offspring to an isolation tank as soon as possible. If the parents had very large offspring in their tank from a previous brood, we allowed them to hatch and keep a second clutch, as the offspring from the two broods could be easily distinguished according to their large size difference (this occurred only in 2 pairs). In 4 pairs a pair member died when already offspring were present and these offspring were removed and stored in a separate tank until personality testing, and the dead partner was replaced with a new partner.

Cross-breeding pairs ( pairs that produced at least one clutch, includes 17 pairs from [46]) were kept in 54 or 58 litre tanks (60 × 30 × 33 cm, 30 cm; or 60 × 30 × 35 cm, 32 cm). However, they included repeated measures of the same female with different males, and vice versa, so in total 19 different females and 20 different males were involved. The pairs were left together to breed for one and a half months. In between they were remeasured and reallocated to different mates. In total, we attempted to mate each individual with 5 different mates, but were only successful (i.e., the pair produced at least one clutch in 1.5 months) for a maximum of 4 different mates (females: 7 × 1, 8 × 2, 1 × 3 and 3 × 4 mates; males: 7 × 1, 9 × 2, 3 × 3 and 1 × 4 mates), partly because we lost and had to replace 4 individuals intermittently. All clutches were removed into 24-litre tanks (Figure 1(a), clutch treatment “isolation”), penultimate clutches were used for the clutch treatment “with parents” (in their 54 or 58 litre tanks) or “with foster parents” (see below), to increase the sample sizes for these latter two treatments.

Cross-fostering pairs ( pairs that produced at least one clutch, in one pair their single clutch did not hatch) were kept in 54 or 58 litre tanks (60 × 30 × 33 cm, 30 cm; or 60 × 30 × 35 cm, 32 cm). Their first clutch was removed into a different empty 54 or 58 litre tank (Figure 1(a), clutch treatment “cross-fostering”, see below for details), however, if this clutch did not hatch, the second clutch received the same treatment. All other clutches were removed into the treatment “isolation” (24 litre tank). Pairs kept producing clutches until they were transferred as foster parents to a foster clutch (see Clutch treatments below).

2.3. Clutch Treatments

All pairs were checked every day for new broods. Upon detection of a new clutch, we commenced with a 15 min brood care observation, counting the frequency of cleaning the eggs (each mouth movement counted) and fanning the eggs (aerating the eggs by vibrating with body and fins [67]) for the female and the male separately [47, 66, 68]. The minimum distance to the eggs in cm (with 0 indicating inside the pot(s) containing the eggs) was also noted for the female and male separately. Unfortunately, pairs could also lay clutches under/behind the filter or on the aquarium walls, and these clutches were sometimes not detected until the fry hatched (which occurs 2 to 3 days after egg laying). These cases account for some missing data on clutch size, average egg mass, hatching success (but not the number of hatched offspring, as fry were immediately counted), and brood care behaviour. After the brood care observation, the pot was removed and we counted the number of eggs (clutch size) and measured the average dry egg mass (by sampling up to 5 eggs per clutch and weighing them after 32 h drying in a 70°C oven). Data on brood care, clutch size, and egg mass will be treated in detail elsewhere.

The broods in the treatment “with parents” broods were then immediately placed back with their parents (Figure 1(a)). The broods in the treatments “isolation” and “with foster-pair” broods were permanently removed (Figure 1(a), the pair received a new clean flower-pot halve) and placed into an isolation net inside a separate 24-litre tank (“isolation”) or 54/58 litre tank (“with foster-pair”), and the eggs were incubated using an air stone. Approximately five days after hatching they were released from their isolation net. “Isolation” broods were kept in their 24-litre tanks with their siblings until 55 to 114 days after egg laying, when they were transferred to a bigger tank (34 to 188 litre) to accommodate both their size and numbers (as numbers were highly variable at transfer, ranging between 2 and 67 siblings, we also used highly variable tank sizes). Two broods with a single offspring each and three broods with two offspring each remained in their original tanks, as transfer was not necessary due to their low number and limitations in the availability of bigger tanks. “With fosters” brood were kept together with their siblings in their 54/58-litre tank, until they received a foster pair from day 65 onwards (Figure 1(a)).

2.4. Body Measurements and Personality Testing

Before the personality test (on day −2 before testing) or mate exchange (on day 0 of release with the new mate), each individual was sexed (external papilla inspection under a dissecting microscope), measured (standard length SL and total length TL in 0.1 mm using a dissecting microscope) and weighed (body mass in mg), and fin clipped for a DNA sample and for male/female identification within pairs. All offspring produced in the period September 2009 to March 2010 were tested in 2010, their parents were tested in March 2010 (5 females had lost their mate before March 2010, so 5 pairs had missing data for the male parent; 1 male had lost his mate before March 2010, so 1 pair had missing data for the female parent). The reasoning for testing all offspring was that they had experienced a very prolonged time with their parents, so might be well suited to serve as a benchmark for future studies. Offspring produced in the period March to August 2010 were identified by their clutch identifier and clutch treatment and tested on day 120 since the clutch was laid (Figure 1(a): the six largest siblings, or less if less were alive). An additional sample of the next 6 largest siblings was taken for tests on day 150 after clutch production for all “with parents” treatments, “with foster-parents” treatments, and the first clutch of the “isolation” treatment (Figure 1(a)). We were not able to take additional samples for all second and later clutches of the “isolation” treatments due to time constraints.

The personality testing procedure has been outlined in detail in [4, 39]. Briefly, boldness and aggressiveness tests were conducted for each single focal fish inside a 42-litre tank (Figures 1(b) and 1(c), 50 × 30 × 30 cm; 28 cm water level). In total, 34 of such tanks were available for testing. Focal fish were left for two days to acclimate and settle territory around a single flower pot halve (placed 30 cm from the front glass). In the aggressiveness test, a mirror was placed along one side (Figure 1(b)). Here the total time hiding (in seconds) and aggressive behaviours towards the mirror image were noted: restrained aggression (frequency of slow approach, fast approach, head down display, spread fins, s-bending) and overt aggression (frequency of contact with the mirror, again 5 min total test duration). In the boldness test, a novel object (Figure 1(c), plastic beetle, plastic funnel, plastic blue half moon, clay bird, or plastic white cross) was placed in a front corner (left or right), and the latency to approach this object (in seconds) plus the closest distance to the object (in cm) was recorded (5 min total test duration). The exploration test was conducted in a 400-litre tank, where the fish were left in a partitioned area with a flower pot half for ten minutes before testing (Figure 1(d), so-called “home compartment”). Then the partition wall was removed and the focal fish could start exploring the unknown part of the tank where ten other flower pot halves were placed (“exploration compartment”, Figure 1(d)). Here the time spent moving outside the pots in any compartment, the latency before leaving the home compartment (in seconds), and for the exploration compartment, the latency before entering the first pot, the number of pots approached, the number of pots entered, and the number of different pots entered (0–10) were recorded (again 5 min total test duration). The three tests (boldness, aggressiveness, and exploration propensity) were conducted in randomized order. The three tests were repeated one day later, or rarely on the same day or up to four days later (due to time constraints and tank constraints). Note that in 2005 and 2006 all three tests were conducted in the 400-litre tank and the exploration test lasted 10 min (instead of 5 min, observer Roger Schürch, see [39]). Moreover, due to severe time constraints, the exploration test could not be conducted for all focal animals in 2010, as it involved a lot of time lost in handling fish. See the description of statistical analyses for details on how we have dealt with these differences in procedures.

2.5. Statistical Analyses

We used Categorical Principal Components analyses CatPCA with two-knot spline transformations [69] to summarise the three different tests (boldness, aggressiveness, and exploration propensity) into a single measure of “behavioural type” (object scores, see also [4]). To account for the observer effects, each CatPCA was run separately for each observer (2005-2006: Roger Schürch series of three tests, 2007: Susan Rothenberger series of three tests, 2008: Sebastian Keller/Estée Bochud/Liana Lasut series of three tests, boldness/aggressiveness/exploration tests; 2010: Markus Zöttl tests, including 264 tests where only aggressiveness was recorded; Noémie Chervet series of two tests and Dik Heg series of two tests: aggressiveness and boldness). Noémie Chervet and Dik Heg also conducted exploration tests, but these were excluded from the analyses to keep the data amongst the individuals consistent. This procedure has the advantage that first, all observers automatically scale to a mean “behavioural type” of zero; and second, the different test procedures are also scaled to a mean “behavioural type” of zero (i.e., in 2005-2006 all three tests were conducted inside a 400-litre tank and the exploration tests lasted 10 min versus in 2007–2010 the three tests were conducted according to Figures 1(b)1(d) and always lasted 5 min). This procedure only assumes that all observers capture more or less the complete variation in behavioural types present in the population, which is a reasonable assumption considering the large number of tests each observer conducted, and that all original variables had very high correlations both before and after transformations in the CatPCA, for each observer separately.

Repeatability was estimated using the VARCOMP and RELIABILITY procedures in SPSS 17 [70], by extracting the variance components and the corresponding intraclass correlation coefficients (= repeatability) by using the Restricted Maximum Likelihood method (REML). The procedure was run once for the complete data set using VARCOMP (Restricted Maximum Likelihood Method REML) and once for each time difference between the first test and the next test(s) , where individuals were tested up to 6 six different times using the RELIABILITY procedure (which was easier to use in the latter analyses for data management reasons). Time differences between test and the first test was calculated in days and the repeatability was calculated for days = 0 (test conducted on the same day as the first test), 1, 2, 3, 4, 15 (between 11 and 20 days), 25 (between 21 and 30 days), 35 (between 31 and 40 days), 45 (between 41 and 50 days), 55 (between 51 and 60 days), 90, 120, 150, 175 (between 151 and 200 days), 225 (between 201 and 250 days), and 930 days (between 732 and 1201 days). To estimate the change in repeatability over time, these 16 estimates of repeatability (from 0 to 732–1201 days) were regressed against ln (days + 1) weighted by their sample size (weighted linear regression). Similarly, change in the test scores (test minus the first test) were analysed by regressing this difference against ln (days + 1).

Heritability was estimated using (1) the mid-parent versus mid-offspring weighted regression slope (weighted by the square root of the number of offspring tested [71]). (2) The intraclass correlation coefficients derived from the variance components extracted using the VARCOMP procedure in SPSS 17 for random effects of the pair identity, mother identity, and father identity, respectively, using the REML method. These estimates were verified by using the minimum norm unbiased estimator method, using both the priors 0 or 1 (MINQUE(0) or MINQUE(1) method: see [70]), and since the MINQUE estimates were virtually identical to the REML estimates only the latter are given. Finally, fixed effects on the brood level were tested by General Linear Mixed Models, using as random effects pair identity and brood identity nested within pairs (and extracting the variance components accordingly). The following fixed effects were tested: female stock (1996, 2006, 2009), male stock (1996, 2006, 2009), treatment of the brood (with parents, with foster parents, isolated), volume of the tank in litres (both before and after transfer, if offspring were not transferred volumes were identical), temperature of the tank in degrees Celcius (both before and—if this applies after transfer), and body size of the focal offspring (SL mm). Note that the fixed effects were measured on the brood identity level, and therefore varied between broods within pairs, and that the offspring varied in body sizes (both within and between broods).

Due to replacement of dead mates, we ended with a total sample size of 74 pairs, 162 broods, and 1327 offspring tested for the heritability analyses (from 49 individual females and 50 individual males), in one pair the mother was not tested and in three pairs the father was not tested.

3. Results

3.1. Categorical Principal Component Extraction of Behavioural Types

In total 1779 individuals were tested two to six times for their behavioural traits (on average 2.41 tests per individual or 4290 tests in total). Categorical Principal Components analyses were run for each observer separately, and in each case a single factor was extracted with an Eigenvalue higher than 1 explaining a high proportion of the correlated behaviours in the one to three tests conducted (Table 1). The extracted factor scores were saved as the “behavioural type” of the individual in each test (for the repeatability analyses) or averaged per individual over all their tests as “behavioural type” (for the heritability analyses).

3.2. Repeatability of the Behavioural Types

VARCOMP analysis (REML) showed significant repeatabilities of the behavioural types ( tests of 1779 individuals): the variance attributed to the individuals was 0.5495, the error variance was 0.4576, which gives a repeatability (intraclass correlation coefficient) of = 0.5457 (±0.0149 SE, standard error of the estimate). However, by comparing the test results pairwise with the first test result over time (from 0 days between two tests up to 732–1201 days between two tests) it became clear that the repeatability significantly declined over time (Figure 2(a); regression analysis weighted by sample size: ln[days between tests + 1]; , , , pairwise estimates, ± SE). Both the intercept and the slope of this regression line (depicted in Figure 2(a)) were significantly different from zero ( and −76.6, resp., both ).

Although the repeatability changed over time, the actual behavioural type test scores changed very little over time (Figure 2(b)). On a short-term basis, individuals became bolder, more aggressive, and explorative compared to their first test score, but this difference to the first test score rapidly diminished over time and approached the “no difference” (marked by the red line in Figure 2(b): ). These changes were modeled with a regression analysis (black line in Figure 2(b): change in score ln[days between tests + 1]; , , , , ± SE of the estimates, both the intercept and the slope of this regression line were significantly different from zero: and −5.4, resp., both ).

3.3. Heritability of Behavioural Types

The raw data of the offspring behavioural type scores are given in Figure 3 and descriptive data are provided in Table 2. First, we estimated heritability using weighted regression equations (weighing the regression analysis by offspring number; Figure 4, Table 2): that is, the mid-parent versus mid-offspring behavioural types (Figure 4(a)), mother versus mid-offspring behavioural types (Figure 4(b)), and father versus mid-offspring behavioural types (Figure 4(c)). Heritabilities were significant but low for the mid-parent and mother versus offspring regressions, and nonsignificant for the father-offspring regression (Table 3). Moreover, the heritability estimated from sibling comparisons was high and significant (last row in Table 3).

We should like to point out that the regression approach is inferior to the variance components method: first, because the regression approach assumes the behavioural type of the parents is measured without error; second, because the mixed nature of the data can be better accommodated by the variance components (using the REML method) and this method can be extended to estimate fixed effects (GLMM REML method, see below); third, because the above analyses suggest strong brood identity effects (last row of Table 3), which should be estimated as a random effect nested within pair identity effects (again using the GLMM REML method). We therefore recalculated heritability using the GLMM REML, once with the pair identity or parent’s identity (female or male) as random effects (first three rows in Table 4) and once by adding also a brood identity nested within pair identity as random effects (last three rows in Table 4). Heritability estimates were all significant and now slightly larger than the previous estimates (cf. Table 4 with Table 3).

Interestingly, brood identity random effects remained significant when nested within their parent(s) (last three rows in Table 4). This suggests siblings from the same brood shared a common (environmental) effect on their behavioural type. To explore potential shared effects, we added single fixed effects to a base model containing random effects of pair identity and brood identity nested within pair (Table 5). However, none of these effects significantly affected the behavioural types of the siblings: treatment of the brood, stocks of their parents, tank volumes and water temperatures during raising, and also offspring body size at testing were all clearly nonsignificant (Table 5).

4. Discussion

There are three main results. First, repeatability of behavioural type significantly declined over time, that is, for two tests conducted on the same day repeatability was 0.83, and for two tests conducted up to 1201 days apart repeatability was only 0.19. This time period spans the entire expected maximum life-span of this species, which has been estimated to be ca. 1000 days [63]. Second, heritability was low but significant and depended on the random effect fitted (i.e., pair, mother, father, or brood identity). Third, a significant random effect of brood identity within pair identity suggests a shared effect on the behavioural types of the broods, which did not depend on (i) the treatment of the broods, (ii) origins of female and male stocks, (iii) volumes and temperatures of tanks used to raise the offspring, and (iv) sizes of the offspring at testing. These three main findings are discussed in more detail below.

Temporal and systematic changes in behavioural type have been reported for various animal populations and for humans (e.g., [20, 33, 37, 40, 7280]), including our study species [39]. Although repeatability is a central role in animal (and human) personality research [7], it remains understudied. Clearly, if the repeatability is very low and also fluctuates or changes systematically over time, this would make the interpretation of individual differences in behavioural type liable to criticism and diminish the relevance of the underlying genetical effects. In our study, repeatability diminished strongly over time (Figure 2(a)), but nevertheless the behavioural type scores between two tests were quite comparable (Figure 2(b)). If two tests were conducted only a short time apart (e.g., on the same day), individuals were typically bolder, more aggressive, and explorative during the second test. This strongly suggests a training effect on the test scores of the individuals: a habituation effect which diminishes over time (see [36] for a similar example). Accordingly, if the two tests were conducted widely apart in time, test scores of individuals were on average very similar to each other. We note as a point of criticism that the change in repeatability over time was modeled by us using a simple regression model. However, it is quite likely (as the data in Figure 2(a) suggest) that the repeatability actually stabilizes to a level of 0.4 to 0.5 after 150 days between two tests. A “break-point regression” approach would be a way forward to study this, but would also need more repeatability data from day 150 onwards. We urge scientists to study the repeatability of behavioural types in more depth, as it plays a critical role in the concept of animal personality research, and we concur that these studies are particularly lacking in fish [39, 74, 75, 8185].

We found that the heritability of behavioural type was ca. 0.15 in N. pulcher, which is a rather low estimate compared to other studies (see meta-analysis in [16]: mean = 0.31). However, heritability estimates may critically depend on the variance components which were actually tested, that is, whether the study design allowed testing for (permanent) environmental effects, maternal and paternal effects, and maternal additive genetic effects. Studies in domesticated animals have shown that many genetic and nongenetic factors may contribute to behavioural phenotype of the offspring (e.g., [8691]). Similarly, we found strong evidence for shared sibling environmental effects (brood with pair effects in Table 3) and maternal/paternal effects on behavioural type (discrepancies between pair versus mother versus father effects in Table 3). This suggests that an animal model statistical analysis of the behavioural types in N. pulcher might be a worthwhile enterprise in the future, to disentangle these variance components. We found no effects of some important aspects of the offspring’s rearing environment on their behavioural type (like temperatures, tank sizes, and clutch treatments).

It is yet unknown how behavioural type (e.g., aggressive propensity) matches the behaviour shown under natural conditions (e.g., regarding dominance [9295], territory acquisition [94, 96, 97], mate acquisition [59, 98102], and mating performance [103]; for review see [85, 104]). It would be of particular interest to know how the behavioural type affects fitness and therefore is subject to natural selection. Field studies show that fitness effects of behavioural type may vary over time [105107] and space [37, 94, 108119], and behavioural type scores may not match one to one with actual behaviour shown in nature (e.g., due to context dependence, [120, 121]). Similarly, although exploration propensity is part of the behavioural syndrome in N. pulcher under laboratory conditions, it appears decoupled from the syndrome under natural conditions (i.e., actual distances moved in field settings: [43]). Under seminatural settings, shy fish have more socially positive interactions with their neighbourhood than bold fish, which is contrary to expectation [4]. However, as expected, bold fish are the hotspots of an aggressiveness network [4]. The relevance of all these effects for fitness in N. pulcher remain unclear, as (i) the effects of behavioural type are always small compared to other effects known to affect the behaviour and fitness of N. pulcher (e.g., social status, body size, and sex); (ii) frequencies of aggression, affiliation, and submission do not scale one to one on the behavioural type of the focal individual (Rothenberger et al. manuscript in preparation); (iii) effects of behavioural type on fitness (survival and reproduction) have not yet been measured in the field.

In domesticated and laboratory animals behavioural types (e.g., aggressiveness) are directly subjected to artificial selection by the experimenters [60, 122136], for instance in order to reduce injury risk, fear, or anxiety in the animals, or when they serve as animal model systems. However, in natural populations it is yet unclear to which extent behavioural types are subject to natural phenotypic selection, or to which extent they are coselected with other traits under direct selection (e.g., age at maturity). Therefore, we consider it to be of prime importance for future studies to (i) map standardized behavioural test results (e.g., of aggressive propensity) on actual behaviour shown in nature under all relevant contexts (e.g., aggressiveness measured during all life-stages and types of contests), and (ii) obtain estimates about how different behavioural types relate directly (or indirectly) to fitness in the natural situation. Finally, our results suggest other (non)genetic effects affecting the behavioural type in N. pulcher, which should be analysed in more detail in the future.

Acknowledgments

The authours thank the editors for inviting us to contribute to this special issue. They thank Susan Rothenberger for behavioural tests performed in 2007, Liana Lasut, Esteé Bochud and Sebastian Keller for behavioural tests performed in 2008, and Kristina Sefc and an anonymous reviewer for their comments. This study was supported by SNSF grants 30100A0-122511 to M. Taborsky and 3100A0-108473 to D. Heg. They declare that the results presented in this paper have not been published previously.