Abstract

Local adaptation is the first step in the process of ecological speciation. It is, however, an unstable and dynamic situation. It can be strengthened by the occurrence of alleles more specialized to the different habitats or vanish if generalist alleles arise by mutations and increase in frequency. This process can have complicated dynamics as specialist alleles may be much more common and may maintain local adaptation for a long time. Thus, even in the absence of an absolute fitness tradeoff between habitats, local adaptation may persist a long time before vanishing. Furthermore, several feedback loops can help to maintain it (the reinforcement, demographic, and recombination loops). This reinforcement can occur by modifying one of the three fundamental steps in a sexual life cycle (dispersal, syngamy, meiosis), which promotes genetic clustering by causing specific genetic associations. Distinguishing these mechanisms complements the one- versus two-allele classification. Overall, the relative rates of the two processes (specialization and reinforcement) dictate whether ecological speciation will occur.

1. Introduction

The debate over speciation is not new [17] and is a complex subject as speciation represents a “cluster of theories woven from many strands” [7] that can be approached from different angles: sympatry versus allopatry, intrinsic versus extrinsic selection, premating versus postmating isolation, isolation versus adaptation, one-allele versus two-allele mechanisms, primary versus secondary contact, genic versus genomic, and so forth. This diversity of possible approaches can even lead to a synthesis based on the idea of a menu with different possible options for each course [8].

One view has reemphasized Darwin’s view that speciation was about adaptation to different habitats or niches [2, 4, 9, 10]. At a microevolutionary scale, this process often starts within species as local adaptation. This can be the beginning of future divergence and eventually speciation. However, the basic process of local adaptation is often seen as too “preliminary” to stand at the core of a theory for speciation. Indeed, “mere differential adaptation (…) does not constitute species” [1] and hybrid unfitness is often the main selective scenario envisioned to be necessary. Hybrid unfitness can be caused by alternative adaptive peaks in the same habitat, whereas local adaptation usually occurs with a single peak that differs in different habitats, which is quite different, even if not always recognized as such [1, 8, 11]. Of course, both phenomenon are not exclusive and evaluating their relative importance is often controversial [12], as both produce indistinguishable spatial patterns [13]. We will see that the route from local adaptation to speciation has been explored in a somewhat separate theoretical corpus that still has to be fully incorporated to a microevolutionary view of speciation.

Environmental heterogeneity is pervasive and local adaptation is the direct selective consequence of different selection pressures occurring in different places [14]. Local adaptation—the greater average fitness of local individuals compared to immigrants—occurs whenever the “grain” of habitat is sufficiently coarse compared to the scale of dispersal [15, 16]. It has various consequences ranging from niche evolution, evolution of specialization, to ecological speciation. In the context of speciation, local adaptation is a selective context that is universal: it applies to sexual or asexual species alike, contrary to selection pressures caused by hybrid incompatibilities. However, it also has specific features. First it is a dynamic and gradual process. Local adaptation can constantly strengthen if new alleles better adapted to local conditions arise and increase in frequency, or disappear if generalist genotypes spread or if differentiation is swamped by gene flow. In contrast, a situation of secondary contact is not a gradual process, but often the abrupt exposure of genetic incompatibilities that have accumulated independently in allopatry. It results in tension zones that can be stable over long periods of time [17]. Second, hybrids between two locally adapted parental genotypes are often not the worst genotype in any of the environments. With local adaptation, there is not selection against hybrids per se: there is selection against alleles or genotypes that are not locally favorable. This distinction has important consequences understanding how reinforcement works in the context of local adaptation compared to the case of reinforcement to avoid producing unfit hybrids. Here, I use reinforcement sensu lato to mean the evolution of premating isolation resulting from selection against hybrids or locally maladapted genotypes [18]. In this paper, I will focus on these two topics and try to show that their outcomes are often less straightforward than early models have suggested, and that the theory of local adaptation has to be more fully integrated into a global view of speciation and diversification.

2. Topic 1—The Dynamics of Local Adaptation

Because niche specialization can occur in the presence of gene flow in primary contact and does not require time in isolation to evolve genetic incompatibilities, it results in a more dynamical process, where further niche specialization may or may not occur, with direct consequences on reinforcement towards more isolation.

2.1. Habitats

Models of local adaptation and ecological speciation most often start by assuming that there are different habitats exchanging migrants (with the rate of migration corresponding to the cases of sympatry, parapatry and allopatry [3]). However, the definition of habitat can be problematic. Field ecologists would certainly define it by a combination of biotic/abiotic conditions in a landscape. For instance, when thinking about land plants, they would categorize soil type, moisture, temperature, light, slope, disturbance regime, and so forth. From an ecological genetics perspective, a first difficulty is to define these variations at the relevant scale, that is, at the scale of dispersal of the focal species [15, 16]. A second difficulty is to account for distance. Because dispersal is most often distance limited, the spatial configuration makes a difference in terms of habitat definition. This is well known in complex or mosaic habitats [19], but is true even in very simple landscapes [20]. A given ecological condition will be less prone to local adaptation if close to a habitat boundary than if surrounded by identical conditions. Boundaries may even favor the local evolution of generalists, something that contradicts the simple competition exclusion principle [20]. Finally, conditions also vary through time, which strongly limit the scope for the long term maintenance of locally specialized genotypes. Overall, locally adapted genotypes are unlikely to arise “on every bush” [21], yet are expected to arise frequently.

2.2. Local Adaptation and the Origin of Tradeoffs

One basic process by which local adaptation arises is when, within a large enough habitat, an allele increases in frequency that is beneficial inside but deleterious/neutral outside this habitat. The condition for this increase in frequency is determined by the strength of selection inside and outside, the size of the pocket relative to the scale of dispersal and by possible gene flow asymmetries between habitats [15, 22]. Any allele with a sufficient benefit inside will increase in frequency no matter if it presents strong deleterious effects outside. This process does not necessarily favor alleles exhibiting little antagonistic effect across habitats; anything goes that is sufficiently favorable locally. At the same locus, allele replacement can occur in both directions, favoring either stronger or weaker specialization [23]. Another possibility is that neutral mutations drift at high frequency locally despite being deleterious elsewhere [14, 24, 25]. However, even if this can occur [26], it requires very limited gene flow and may be globally less conducive to strong local adaptation.

This process has no reason to stop and lead to a process of “amelioration” [27] whereby new favorable alleles replace previous ones at a given locus [23, 2830], modifier alleles at new loci evolve to correct for deleterious side effects of previous ones [3133], duplications and new functions can arise [3436], and so forth. The question is whether this amelioration will lead to the evolution of generalist genotypes that can accommodate all habitats or whether local adaptation will strengthen and lead to specialized ecotypes that have diverged at a large number of loci. The answer is not straightforward. The first approach is to build a model imposing a trade-off curve between habitats. Whether specialists or generalists evolve depends then on the shape of this trade-off curve [37, 38]. Globally speaking, more concave curves facilitates the evolution of specialists, whereas more convex ones favor generalists, and sometimes both can coexist [37, 38]. However, there is no clear ultimate reason for choosing one curve over another or not allowing these trade-off curves to evolve as well. Another approach that has been much less explored would be to introduce a distribution of mutation effects, where specialist mutations are much more common than generalist mutations (having to solve the problem of a single habitat) and are constantly appearing at different loci maintaining local adaptation. In the latter situation, and if no other constraint is involved, the outcome in the very long run would be nearly perfect adaptation to the different habitats. There are however three positive feedback loops that are likely to interfere with this outcome and favor increased specialization.

2.3. The Demographic Feedback Loop

The first feedback is demographic. As far as local adaptation causes a local increase in density, it will also make life easier for more specialist alleles to increase in frequency. This is due to the fact that density differences cause asymmetric gene flow, which gives an advantage to alleles favored in the denser habitat [14, 22]. Because of gene flow, an allele too detrimental outside the habitat where it is favored may be unable to increase in frequency. Despite having the potential to contribute to specialization if the habitat was isolated, it remains at mutation-selection balance. Such alleles may be very common; I term them “contending” alleles. If density becomes higher in this habitat, contending alleles may now be able to increase in frequency and contribute to local adaptation. This increase in density is likely to occur at least in some cases when local adaptation takes place. The positive feedback loop occurs because a local increase in density causes more and more alleles that are locally beneficial to be recruited, which strengthens local adaptation, increases local density and facilitates further the increase in frequency of other locally beneficial alleles (the reverse can also occur, which is known as migration meltdown [14, 39]). Because the ratio of density is as effective as the square of selection ratio inside versus outside the habitat [14, 22], this effect is likely to be strong in natural populations. Conversely adapting to a sink habitat makes it very difficult for the same reason as shown by niche expansion models [4042].

2.4. The Recombination Feedback Loop

The second feedback loop is due to indirect selection among loci directly involved in local adaptation. When a locally beneficial allele increases in frequency, it will favor the spread at closely linked loci of other locally beneficial alleles. This is due to the fact that dispersal generates linkage disequilibrium between loci that share a similar frequency variation across habitats, as expected for two loci involved in local adaptation to the same habitat. This linkage disequilibrium translates into indirect selection that mutually benefits the locally adapted alleles at the two loci [43, 44]. For instance, a contending allele could start to increase in frequency if it becomes sufficiently linked to another locus involved in the local adaptation. This phenomenon generates a positive feedback loop within the genome where locally adapted alleles are more likely to be recruited in genomic regions already harboring a previous locus involved in the local adaptation. It can generate “genomic islands” of local adaptation that extend further and further [45, 46], a specific process that can gradually produce strong genetic divergence at many contiguous loci in linkage disequilibrium [1, 47].

2.5. The Reinforcement Feedback Loop

The third feedback loop is due to reinforcement, that is, the evolution of traits promoting premating isolation between differentially locally adapted genotypes. Reinforcement tends to make life easier for locally beneficial alleles: it allows more alleles contributing to local adaptation to be recruited and locally beneficial alleles to reach higher frequencies. For instance, contending alleles could be recruited if habitat choice started to evolve. In effect, habitat choice minimizes the possible negative fitness that an allele can have in habitats that are different from the habitat where it is favored. Thus, reinforcement is likely to promote increased specialization. Reciprocally, strong local adaptation increases the selection pressure to reinforce it, so that both phenomena can act in concert in a positive feedback loop [9, 37, 38, 48].

2.6. The Relative Dynamics of Local Adaptation and Reinforcement

Ultimately, ecological speciation will result only if reinforcement occurs quickly enough compared to the evolution of generalists and the breakdown of local adaptation. The different feedback loops mentioned above will tend to favor this outcome, but may not be strong enough to lead to speciation. For instance, the evolution of habitat choice, reduced dispersal, selfing, and so forth, can strongly reduce the chance that a generalist allele would spread, but the actual outcome depends on the relative rates of reinforcement and loss of specialization. This dynamical issue is not something that has been fully appreciated in the context of ecological speciation (but see [38] in the context of a fixed tradeoff). It differs from the situation of reinforcement in a tension zone by the fact that it is less stable and very sensitive to several feedbacks. More work is certainly needed to clearly delineate the conditions favoring speciation in this context.

3. Topic 2—The Reinforcement of Local Adaptation

Individuals that have parents selected in the same habitat and that stay in that habitat are more likely to have genes appropriate to that environment than another randomly chosen individual. Thus, mating locally and staying in the same habitat is always favored from the point of view of a continually evolving genome” [25].

In the early 70s, several models have addressed the problem of reinforcement of local adaptation. The first was proposed by Antonovics [49]. This model showed that, because mating at random is risky in the context of local adaptation, evolution favors that like mates with like and that selfing is even safer to maintain local adaptation. Then Balkau and Feldman [50] showed that, in the context of local adaptation, migrating, or sending offspring elsewhere is likely to decrease an individual’s fitness or that of its offspring. With a modifier model, they showed that this effect caused indirect selection to reduce dispersal as much as possible. Finally, Slatkin [43] suggested and D. Charlesworth and B. Charlesworth confirmed [51] that sex and recombination is likely to break combination of genes that have been locally selected for and should be selected against in the context of local adaptation. These findings have since been constantly reported or given as examples of “one-allele” mechanisms that are likely to drive the evolution of isolation in parapatry. (The one-allele versus two-allele classification refers to cases where a single or two different alleles spread at the modifier locus to promote genetic clustering [21]. This classification is very useful despite leading to some complications (see [52] and Table 1 note 7)). In this section, I will reconsider these conclusions in the light of more recent models on the evolution of assortative mating [53], dispersal [54, 55], and recombination [11, 45, 56] in the context of local adaptation. Contrary to what is commonly thought [8, 21, 57], I will show that these one-allele mechanisms do not inevitably lead to speciation, even in the absence of direct cost. I will then make a comparison of the underlying mechanisms and propose a typology of cases (orthogonal to the one- versus two-allele classification) that may prove useful understanding and modeling speciation (Table 1).

Before proceeding, we note that several important findings have also been made regarding this process since these early models. First, the role of ecological-based adaptation in speciation has received considerable support in the last decade [2, 4, 9, 10]. Second several empirical findings have supported the idea that reinforcement could indeed occur in the context of adaptation to different habitats [49, 5867] or at least that there is often ample opportunity for reinforcement [6].

3.1. The Evolution of Selfing and Assortative Mating

The evolution of nonrandom mating has been extensively studied in the context of reinforcement and reproductive isolation [18, 6870]. However, it has also been extensively studied to understand the evolution of mating systems within species [7173]. Interestingly, the two approaches are usually considered separately and emphasize completely opposite outcomes. The first predicts the evolution of more assortative mating with increased outbreeding depression or hybrid unfitness. The second predicts the evolution of less assortative mating or selfing with increased inbreeding depression. The models studying reinforcement include outbreeding but not inbreeding depression [18, 70, 7477] while the models studying mating system evolution do exactly the opposite [7173].

Local adaptation causes outbreeding depression if different alleles are favored in different habitats [78], which is the reason why it is widely thought that spatially heterogenous selection favors the evolution of assortative mating by a one-allele mechanism in the absence of direct costs [8, 18, 21, 68, 69, 79, 80]. However, the dominance relationship of locally adapted alleles within each habitat may also cause inbreeding depression which can, in fact, prevent the evolution of premating isolation. When both phenomena act in concert, the outcomes vary tremendously depending on parameter values [53], and more assortment is not necessarily favored even in presence of strong local adaptation. In fact, because a polymorphism at a locus involved in local adaptation is more easily maintained when locally beneficial alleles are dominant, less assortment may often be favored in natural situations with local adaptation [53]. There are theoretical reasons for expecting such dominance relationships between locally adapted alleles [81]. Because theoretical models of reinforcement have rarely considered the case of local adaptation, and when they did, considered only haploid or diploid with particular dominance [18, 70, 82], these conclusions have remained largely overlooked. There are numerous mechanisms of assortment [69] and each can evolve slightly differently. For instance when local adaptation is based on a conspicuous trait (shell thickness in Littorina [65], coloration in Chrysopa [62] or Heliconius [83], etc.), mate choice can be cued directly on this trait, which is very efficient unless the right mate is rare and difficult to find in the population [84]. Another simple way to mate with a self-similar phenotype is to self-fertilize when hermaphrodite which is also very efficient, does not incur the cost of finding the right mate and does not require the ability to discriminate the locally adapted trait. In both cases, local adaptation loci will cause indirect selection (Figure 1) and, if not codominant, inbreeding depression. With selfing however, all other loci in the genome experiencing recessive deleterious mutations will also contribute to inbreeding depression. As a consequence, and unless the cost of finding a mate is high, reinforcement may be less likely to evolve via selfing than via assortment based on the local adaptation traits [53]. Two-allele models and models involving sex-specific traits and preferences also provide several alternatives [8, 52].

3.2. The Evolution of Dispersal

Individuals that have survived until reproduction have genotypes that work relatively well where they are. Because of environmental heterogeneity, migrating or sending offspring elsewhere is likely to decrease fitness. Local adaptation indeed generates an indirect selection pressure in favor of less dispersal [50, 8587] (Figure 2). However, as for the evolution of nonrandom mating, the evolution of dispersal has been studied in a variety of contexts and not only in reference to the process of the reinforcement of local adaptation or speciation and a large number of factors interact to shape this trait [88]. However, in the context of the evolution of dispersal in presence of local adaptation there are at least two factors that cannot be ignored. The first is that, as in the case of the evolution of assortment, inbreeding depression causes a selection pressure in favor of dispersal [89]. This inbreeding depression can be partly, but not only, caused by the loci responsible for the local adaptation. The second factor is kin selection (Figure 3). As soon as one considers a stochastic model for the evolution of dispersal, kin selection occurs and must be taken into account to determine how dispersal evolves [9092]. Intuitively, it is straightforward to see that a given allele causing zero dispersal cannot fix in a subdivided population. In other words, zero dispersal cannot be a convergent stable state as was suggested in deterministic models of reinforcement. As expected from this heuristic argument, kin selection favors more dispersal than predicted in a deterministic model [54]. However, this is not the only effect as local adaptation interacts with the effect of kin selection: strong differentiation at a local adaptation locus magnifies kin selection at short recombination distance. This indirect kin selection can cause bistability (i.e., different dispersal rate can evolve depending on the initial conditions), which changes qualitatively the expectation [54]. There are different ways to reduce dispersal, and all may not be equivalent even if the selection pressures at work will share strong similarities. In particular it is clearly important to distinguish between dispersal and habitat choice. As we have seen dispersal cannot evolve to very low rates because of kin selection. However, choosing the natal habitat (to maintain local adaptation) while quitting its natal patch (to release kins from competition) may provide the best from both worlds and is therefore a more likely candidate trait for reinforcement. Two-allele models also provide several alternatives [38, 57].

3.3. Comparisons among Reinforcement Traits

The common effect in all these processes is that alleles that favor more assortment, less dispersal or tighter linkage become associated with locally beneficial alleles, which in turn generates an indirect selection pressure in their favor. In each of these cases however, the way linkage disequilibrium is built between the modifiers and the locally beneficial alleles is distinct (compare Figures 1, 2 and 4). First, a modifier has an immediate effect on the genetic composition of the population, here genotypic frequencies at the local adaptation loci: dispersal modification changes allelic frequencies; assortment changes within locus associations; recombination changes between loci associations. This immediate effect causes a frequency change at the modifier locus if there is selection on alleles, dominance, and epistasis, respectively. When the modifier changes within or between loci associations, a secondary effect occurs. Increased associations generate a higher variance in fitness, more efficient selection, and thus an increase of the frequency difference between habitats at the local adaptation locus. As a consequence, modifiers increasing these associations (modifier increasing assortment or reducing recombination in our examples) become associated, and hitchhike, with locally beneficial alleles. There are thus several ways to promote distinct genetic clusters between habitats and the three examples detailed in this paper illustrate each of these cases: directly magnifying allelic frequency differences between populations (case illustrated by dispersal modification), promoting within locus associations (case illustrated by assortment modification), promoting between loci associations (case illustrated by recombination modification). Reinforcement may occur by the evolution of many other traits than the ones mentioned here (in particular involving two-allele mechanisms, see Table 1), but their impact are likely to be achieved via one of these effects alone or in combination. Considering the three possible impacts of a modifier on the genetic composition of populations (on frequencies, within locus and between loci associations) may be a useful typology to understand the different ways reinforcement and genetic clustering can occur. It is orthogonal to, and complements the usual one- versus two-allele classification (Table 1).

4. Conclusion

The first conclusion is that the process of reinforcement and local adaptation are intertwined and occur simultaneously. Whether pre- and postzygotic isolation will eventually evolve is uncertain in such a dynamic process. In particular, local adaptation can collapse if generalist alleles arise and spread. However, there are several positive feedback loops that will tend to drive the system towards divergence (the reinforcement, demographic, and recombination loops). From a theoretical standpoint, this process has rarely been analyzed jointly and in a dynamic way with changes in local adaptation itself. In the context of mounting evidence in favor of ecological speciation [106, 107], such an approach would certainly help evaluate its likelihood, tempo, and mechanism.

Second, Felsenstein [21] proposed to distinguish the different mechanisms for reinforcement on the idea that they involved the spread of one or two-alleles in the incipient species. This distinction is an important one, but it is not the only one to be made. Many one-allele mechanisms are only superficially similar as they can promote genetic clustering and speciation in different ways. A useful typology could be that they increase differentiation among populations, heterozygote deficit, or linkage disequilibrium, which corresponds to modifying one of the three fundamental events in a sexual life cycle (dispersal, syngamy, or meiosis, resp.). Furthermore, different traits may increase genetic clustering, but may not contribute to reinforcement because they are exposed to a variety of other selective effects. Models of reinforcement based on the evolution of particular traits must integrate what is known outside the speciation literature for those traits. For instance, recombination [108], mating systems [109], and dispersal [110], as discussed above, have all been intensely studied outside this context pinpointing a variety of selective effects. These theoretical developments certainly have to be merged.

Acknowledgments

I thank A. Whibley, J. Mallet and two anonymous reviewers for comments on this manuscript. This work was supported by the European Research Council starting grant “QuantEvol”.