Abstract

Objective. To examine exercise-induced changes in the reward value of food during medium-term supervised exercise in obese individuals. Subjects/Methods. The study was a 12-week supervised exercise intervention prescribed to expend 500 kcal/day, 5 d/week. 34 sedentary obese males and females were identified as responders (R) or non-responders (NR) to the intervention according to changes in body composition relative to measured energy expended during exercise. Food reward (ratings of liking and wanting, and relative preference by forced choice pairs) for an array of food images was assessed before and after an acute exercise bout. Results. 20 responders and 14 non-responders were identified. R lost 5.2 kg ± 2.4 of total fat mass and NR lost 1.7 kg ± 1.4. After acute exercise, liking for all foods increased in NR compared to no change in R. Furthermore, NR showed an increase in wanting and relative preference for high-fat sweet foods. These differences were independent of 12-weeks regular exercise and weight loss. Conclusion. Individuals who showed an immediate post-exercise increase in liking and increased wanting and preference for high-fat sweet foods displayed a smaller reduction in fat mass with exercise. For some individuals, exercise increases the reward value of food and diminishes the impact of exercise on fat loss.

1. Introduction

The capacity for exercise to reduce overweight varies considerably between individuals [14]. It has been demonstrated that some individuals experience a lower than predicted fat loss despite 90% adherence to 12 weeks of daily supervised exercise [3]. Characterising the determinants of this variability–particularly for the less successful individuals–could help to design more appropriate and effective weight loss strategies. Previously, we reported that weight loss during a program of regular moderate intensity exercise was partly determined by differences in hunger levels experienced during the day [5]. One additional cause of the variability in weight loss could be exercise-induced alterations in the reward value of foods with particular sensory and/or macronutrient profiles [6]. The extent that changes in food reward go on to influence food choice may then determine the degree of compensatory eating [7, 8]. This relationship could depend in part on eating behaviour traits of the population studied such as restrained or disinhibited eating styles [912], or it could be influenced by metabolic processes such as substrate oxidation or secretion of gastrointestinal hormones [1316]. It has been hypothesised that exercise causes a neurochemical response that has a sensitizing action on brain reward systems (e.g., [17]). Another possibility is the deliberate choice of highly palatable, energy-dense foods (e.g., fatty or sweet tasting “treats”) to reward virtuous behavior or regulate changes in mood [18] or stress [19].

Recently, we found in lean individuals that enhanced implicit wanting for images of food after exercise could predict those who also increased their food intake compared to those whose food intake did not change [20]. However, insufficient data are available on the extent to which exercise-induced changes in food reward influence energy intake through macronutrient selection [6], and there are no data on the effect of exercise on food reward when exercise is continued over many weeks. Furthermore, it is unknown whether the reward value of food has a role in the effect of medium-term exercise on energy balance and weight loss in overweight and obese individuals. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine exercise-induced changes in the reward value of food during a medium term (12 weeks), fixed volume schedule of supervised exercise in sedentary overweight and obese individuals. Participants were assessed using a validated computerised procedure [21, 22] that measured components of reward (liking, wanting, and food preference) for an array of photographic food images that varied in taste and fat content, before and after an acute bout of exercise. This procedure was repeated at baseline and week 12 of the intervention.

We predicted that acute exercise would cause an increase in liking, wanting, and preference for high-fat food at the beginning of the intervention. Furthermore, we predicted that after 12 weeks of regular exercise, these effects would be attenuated in those who met or exceeded the predicted reduction in fat mass (Responders) compared to those who compensated for the exercise and experienced a lower than predicted reduction in fat mass (Nonresponders).

2. Methods

2.1. Subjects and Recruitment

Forty sedentary overweight and obese but otherwise healthy volunteers (13 males) with mean body mass index = 31.3 ( 3.8 kg/m2) and age = 39.6 ( 10.5 years) participated in this study. Participants were recruited from those who enrolled on a 12-week exercise programme conducted at the Human Appetite Research Unit, University of Leeds [23]. Therefore, these participants formed a subset of individuals who were taking part in a larger study. The data reported in this study have not been published previously. Participants gave their written consent to take part in the study, and ethical approval was obtained from the Institute of Psychological Sciences Ethics committee.

2.2. Experimental Design

The study examined the acute effects of a bout of exercise on the reward value of food at two time points immediately before and following a 12-week schedule of regular supervised exercise. Participants provided reward measures for 20 photographic images of food presented via computer immediately before and then immediately following a supervised exercise session. The food images varied in taste and macronutrient properties and were categorised according to sensory domain (sweet/non-sweet) and fat/carbohydrate content (high or low). Participants were identified as “responders” or “Nonresponders” to the 12-week intervention according to their final net energy balance which was based on their actual measured changes in body composition relative to the gross exercise-induced energy expenditure.

2.3. Exercise Protocol

The exercise protocol used in this study has been described in detail elsewhere [5, 23]. Participants underwent a 12-week exercise programme that was individually prescribed to expend 500 kcal per session, 5 days per week. Therefore, a total volume of 30,000 kcal energy expenditure was prescribed. All exercise sessions were supervised in the research unit and participants wore a heart rate monitor during each session. The exercise prescription was adjusted for each participant every 4 weeks using submaximal tests during which indirect calorimetry was performed. This allowed the exercise prescription to be modified to account for changes in cardiovascular fitness and body weight. The acute exercise bout fixed was intensity submaximal exercise (70% heart rate max) conducted on a stationary exercise bike (Sapilo, Italy). Water was provided at the beginning and end of the session. Energy expenditure of each exercise bout was calculated by comparing heart rate and duration of the session against expenditure values from indirect calorimetry.

2.4. Body Composition

Body weight and body composition (lean and fat mass) were measured using air displacement plethysmography (BodPod, Life Measurement Incorporated, Concord, CA). ADP has been validated against dual energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) and is more suitable for frequent repeated measures research [2427]. Height was measured using a stadiometer (Seca, Leicester, UK).

2.5. Measurement of Reward Value of Food

The reward value of food was measured using a computer-based behavioural procedure called the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire (LFPQ) [21, 22]. The LFPQ has demonstrated good test-retest reliability, both on immediate repetition and after one week (typical =.61–.95). The procedure has proven sensitive to acute dietary manipulations [28, 29] and single bouts of exercise in nonobese women [20]. Concurrent validity of the LFPQ with other behavioural paradigms of food reward (i.e., using progressive ratio schedules of reinforcement) is satisfactory [30]. In this measure, an array of 20 photographic food images was used within two behavioural tasks administered using experiment generator software (E-prime v.1.2, Psychology Software Tools, ND). The foods were chosen to vary along the dimensions of sensory domain (sweet/non-sweet taste) and fat/carbohydrate content (high or low by percentage). Half the foods contained  45% energy as fat while the other half comprised  20%. These foods were further divided into sweet tasting (i.e., dessert foods) and non-sweet tasting (i.e., salty foods). The foods were controlled between categories for energy and protein content. All foods had been validated in a previous study and rated using 7-point scales on their perceived familiarity, typicality of presentation (most to least typically encountered), and perceived macronutrient and taste properties [22]. See Table 1 for details of the food images used in the LFPQ. Food images were presented individually or in pairs on a monitor and measured  mm2. Participants responses to the foods in the array were pooled according to categories of high-fat non-sweet (HFNS), low-fat non-sweet (LFNS), high-fat sweet (HFSW), and low-fat non-sweet (LFNS). Three separate measures were derived from participants’ evaluation of the foods.

2.6. Expected Liking

Participants were prompted with the question “How pleasant would it be to taste this food now?”. Expected liking measured the conscious feeling of pleasure expected from tasting each food. Foods were rated individually according to a 100 mm visual analogue scale anchored at each end by the statements “not at all” and “extremely”. Scores for each food category were aggregated from the individual ratings with a possible range of 0–100.

2.7. Explicit Wanting

Using the same response format as expected liking, participants responded to the question “How much do you want some of this food now?”. Explicit wanting measures the conscious desire for each food [21, 31]. Scores for each food category ranged from 0 to 100.

2.8. Food Preference

Relative preference for each food category was measured by a series of “forced choice” pairings of each food image with every alternative food in the array. Participants were presented with 150 such pairs and followed the written instruction in each trial to select the food they “most want to eat now”. Foods were randomly presented on the left or right side of the screen and participants could choose between each pair by pressing the corresponding buttons. The number of choices made in each food category (possible range 0–75) was recorded.

2.9. Statistics

Data were prepared using Microsoft Excel and analysed using SPSS v.16 for Windows. Response to the intervention was assessed by calculating net energy balance from measured energy expenditure during the exercise compared to estimated energy flux from measured changes in body composition. We used the estimation that 1 kg loss of fat mass is equivalent to 9,540 kcal, and 1 kg loss of lean mass is equivalent to 1,100 kcal [32]. Therefore, participants who achieved a negative energy balance which matched or exceeded predicted changes in body composition were identified as “Responders”, while those whose actual weight loss was lower than the predicted were identified as “Nonresponders” (indicating a degree of energy compensation). mixed ANOVA were used to compare anthropometric values at baseline and week 12, within- and between-responder and non-responder groups. Changes in the reward value of food were analysed by separate 3-way mixed ANCOVA (Responder group intervention week food category) with baseline BMI as covariate. Interactions between intervention week, food category, and Responder group were verified by -tests with Bonferroni correction to control for type I error.

3. Results

3.1. Identification of Responders and Nonresponders

Thirty six out of 40 participants successfully adhered to the 12-week intervention. Two participants had incomplete LFPQ data. Therefore, data for 34 participants were included in the analyses. The individual variability in energy balance at week 12 is shown in Figure 1. Based on these participants’ net energy balance, we identified 20 Responders (6 males) and 14 Nonresponders (7 males) to the intervention. Measured energy expenditure from exercise did not differ significantly between groups There was a trend for Responders to have a greater BMI at baseline compared to Nonresponders Mean duration of the acute exercise bout was greater at baseline compared to week 12 and mean verified expenditure of the bout was less at baseline than week 12 There were no differences in duration or expenditure between Responders and Nonresponders. The characteristics of these groups are presented in Table 2.

R experienced a greater negative energy balance compared with NR which was reflected in their greater fat mass loss. There were no differences in other variables including starting weight, change in lean mass, duration of exercise sessions, or net energy expenditure from exercise.

3.2. Hedonic Evaluation of Food Images (Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire)

Exercise induced changes on the LFPQ are shown for Responders and Nonresponders in Table 3. ANCOVA revealed no effects of baseline BMI or BMI food category interactions on liking, wanting, or food preference.

3.3. Expected Liking

There was a significant main effect of Responder group on changes in liking [ (1,31) = 12.2, ]. Ratings of liking increased after exercise in NR compared to R. This finding was similar across all food categories and at weeks 0 and 12. That is, the increased liking was independent of food category and after 12 weeks of exercise-induced increase in EE.

3.4. Explicit Wanting

Similar to liking, there was a main effect of Responder group on explicit wanting [ (1,31) = 6.6, ]. In addition, there was a significant interaction between responder group and food category on changes in explicit wanting [ (3,93) = 6.9, ]. NR showed a specific increase in explicit wanting for high-fat sweet foods This effect appeared more pronounced at week 0 (see Figure 3), however, the three-way interaction did not reach significance [ (3,93) = 2.1, ]. The trend for an increase in wanting for high-fat non-sweet foods was nonsignificant after Bonferroni corrections were applied.

3.5. Food Preference

A significant interaction between Responder group and food category on relative preference was revealed [ (3,93) = 6.1, ]. Post hoc tests confirmed that NR showed an increased preference for high-fat sweet food Interestingly, these differences were less pronounced at week 12 compared to week 0. There was no main effect of responder group on food preference [ (1,31) = 1.6, ].

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine the acute effect of exercise on the reward value of food before and after medium-term regular exercise in overweight and obese volunteers. In accordance with previous research [23, 33, 34], there was a large individual variability in net energy balance, indicating some degree of compensation for the 12-week exercise-induced energy expenditure in 14 out of 34 participants. This variability were specifically associated with differences in fat mass. We hypothesised that acute exercise would increase the reward value of food measured at the outset of the intervention, and that after 12 weeks of regular supervised exercise these effects would be dependent on the degree of energy compensation identified by classifying participants as Responders or Nonresponders based on changes in body composition. We found that rather than differences in food reward emerging at week 12, liking and wanting only increased at baseline in the Nonresponders, while the Responders did not change. Furthermore, this pattern of behaviour in both groups was stable over time. These data provide evidence that compensation associated with lower fat loss is associated with the acute effects of exercise on components of food reward involved in appetite regulation and food choice.

Previous research on exercise and the reward value of food has been limited to cross-sectional studies in lean individuals or trained athletes on taste palatability. One study in overweight sedentary participants provides a notable exception [18]. In a recent review, Elder and Roberts [6] identified 9 studies investigating the acute effect of exercise on palatability [8, 10, 3541]. The findings from these studies are not consistent but are generally suggestive of an increase in the perceived pleasantness of foods with a range of sensory and macronutrient profiles. None of the studies found a devaluing effect of exercise on food reward. However, Elder and Roberts concluded that “insufficient data are available on whether changes in food preferences and taste perception influence energy balance through macronutrient selection” (page 1). In the present study, we found that those who did not respond to the exercise intervention as predicted were characterised by differences in the reward value of food after an acute bout of exercise. In addition to increases in expected liking for all food categories independent of taste or fat content, explicit wanting and relative preference for high fat sweet foods were accentuated. These findings are consistent with our previous research on exercise-induced compensatory eating in lean women. Individuals who increased their food intake immediately after 50 minutes of moderate intensity exercise responded faster to images of food, reported greater liking for the food, and had an increased preference toward high-fat sweet food [20].

What mechanisms could account for the differences observed in the reward value of food? One hypothesis can be generated by research proposing that short bouts of exercise may stimulate dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and striatum [17] or in the ventral tegmental area in response to CRF and cortisol [42]. In rats, it was shown that an acute bout of exercise can exert an enhancing effect on reward (similar to a low dose of an addictive drug) via a sensitizing action [17]. Such neural sensitization is thought to involve changes in levels of delta FosB and dopamine transmission in the mesolimbic pathway [43, 44]. It is possible therefore that some individuals experience a form of sensitization to food (and visual food cues) induced by exercise. This could be due to repeated associations between exercise and food seeking behavior that have developed into habit.

Exercise-induced changes in food reward could be an important consideration in the efficacy of exercise as a means to reduce overweight. In particular, an enhanced motivational drive or wanting for food after exercise may help to explain why some people overcompensate during acute eating episodes [20]. The findings of the present study suggest that some individuals have a predisposition to compensate for exercise-induced energy expenditure as a result of changes in food reward. However, they also demonstrate that exercise does not increase the reward value of food in all individuals and may differ according to the characteristics (e.g., macronutrient and sensory profiles) of the food being assessed. This could explain certain discrepancies in the literature on exercise and food palatability [6].

The effects of exercise on food preference can be linked to both the metabolic and cognitive consequences of engaging in physical activity. Furthermore, these domains interact and their impact on appetite regulation will depend on individual predispositions and susceptibility [45]. Future research should aim to identify the characteristics of those individuals who are most likely to respond to regular exercise (see, e.g., [46, 47]). Contrary to our expectations, the differences in liking and wanting immediately after exercise observed between Responders and Nonresponders were present at baseline and remained after 12 weeks of regular supervised exercise. This suggests that hedonic response after exercise may be an enduring predisposition and one that could moderate the effect of exercise on fat loss. In contrast, we observed at week 0 that Nonresponders showed a strong preference for high-fat sweet foods after exercise, but at week 12 the exercise bout had no such effect. With caution, we propose that regular moderate intensity exercise may have a role in correcting the initial preference for high palatability, high-energy food brought about by acute exercise. This interpretation is supported by other 12-week exercise interventions that demonstrate a correcting effect of exercise training on satiety [5] and energy compensation [48].

Some limitations should be noted in the present study. The restricted sample size limited the opportunity to control for numerous background variables or test more complex hypotheses involving combinations of factors. Although water was provided during exercise, we did not measure total consumption; and similarly we did not measure sensations of appetite during the exercise session. Therefore, it was not possible to test whether our results were mediated by hunger, thirst, or differences in water intake. The study design did not include a no-exercise control arm. Although each subject served as their own control before and after the exercise session and before and after the 12-week intervention, we cannot reject the possibility that reward value of food may increase in Nonresponders after the first set of measures regardless of performing exercise. There is evidence to suggest that prior exposure to the sight and smell of food can increase motivation to eat in the short term [49]. Nevertheless, we argue that 500 kcal of moderate intensity exercise is more likely having a greater modulating effect on food reward in Nonresponders than mere exposure to food cues. Moreover, we only assessed the reward value of food once before and after exercise at week 0 and week 12. Although the immediate and one-week test-retest reliability of the LFPQ is acceptable, further verification of the effects we observed would strengthen our interpretation. Another issue is the use of visual food stimuli to assess hedonic evaluation of food. It is unknown whether we would have demonstrated similar effects if we measured liking or wanting and preference for food using more potent sensory cues such as smell, taste, or ingestion. It is possible that Nonresponders would be more able to inhibit their spontaneous responses to food images compared to smelling or tasting the food presented. Similarly, the exercise-induced reduction in liking and wanting for high-fat sweet food in the responders might be accentuated due to greater aversion to stronger sensory cues.

5. Conclusion

To conclude, overweight and obese individuals who showed an immediate postexercise increase in expected liking for food and more specifically an increased wanting and preference for high-fat sweet foods displayed a smaller reduction in fat mass with exercise. Increased liking for foods can promote higher compensation of energy intake in response to exercise. This enhanced liking was not attenuated by prolonged exercise (or change in body composition) suggesting that it is a strong habitual trait. This maintenance of a strong positive rating of liking for foods can, apparently, offset subtle changes in preferences among food groups which tend towards a more healthy eating style. For some individuals, exercise increases the reward value of high palatability, high energy food and diminishes the impact of exercise on fat loss. Early identification of this predisposition could help to optimise weight control strategies by augmenting the health benefits of exercise with dietary modification or pharmacotherapy.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (Grant nos. BBS/B/05079 and BB/G530141/1).