Table of Contents Author Guidelines Submit a Manuscript
Neuroscience Journal
Volume 2016 (2016), Article ID 4962562, 6 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/4962562
Review Article

How Extended Is Wernicke’s Area? Meta-Analytic Connectivity Study of BA20 and Integrative Proposal

1Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, USA
2Radiology Department and Research Institute, Miami Children’s Hospital, Miami, FL 33155, USA
3Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Davie, FL 33144, USA

Received 28 September 2015; Accepted 27 January 2016

Academic Editor: Pasquale Striano

Copyright © 2016 Alfredo Ardila et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Understanding the functions of different brain areas has represented a major endeavor of contemporary neurosciences. The purpose of this paper was to pinpoint the connectivity of Brodmann area 20 (BA20) (inferior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus) in language tasks. A meta-analysis was conducted to assess the language network in which BA20 is involved. The DataBase of Brainmap was used; 11 papers corresponding to 12 experimental conditions with a total of 207 subjects were included in this analysis. Our results demonstrated seven clusters of activation including other temporal lobe areas (BA3, BA21), the insula, and the prefrontal cortex; minor clusters in the cingulate gyrus and the occipital lobe were observed; however, the volumes of all the activation clusters were small. Our results suggest that regardless of BA20 having certain participation in language processes it cannot be considered as a core language processing area (Wernicke’s area); nonetheless, it could be regarded as kind of language processing marginal area, participating in “extended Wernicke’s area” or simply “Wernicke’s system.” It is suggested that “core Wernicke’s area” roughly corresponds to BA21, BA22, BA41, and BA42, while a “language associations area” roughly corresponds to BA20, BA37, BA38, BA39, and BA40 (“extended Wernicke’s area” or “Wernicke’s system”).

1. Introduction

The exact location and extension of Wernicke’s area have been inconsistent and polemic [1]. Conceptually, it is easy to define Wernicke’s area which corresponds to the language auditory processing area in the left hemisphere [24]. Thus, the controversial and unanswered question becomes the following: which are the borders of the auditory language processing area in the left hemisphere?

Since Wernicke [5] the auditory language processing area (later known as “Wernicke’s area”) has been generally, but not always, equated with the first temporal gyrus of the left hemisphere. Similarly, when Dejerine [6] referred to the “language zone” (or “language area”), he included (in addition to Broca’s area involved in language production and the angular gyrus participating in written language) the first temporal gyrus (referred to as “Wernicke’s area”) related to language understanding. During the following decades, the idea of the so-called language area (roughly corresponding to the peri-Sylvian area of the left hemisphere) was solidly integrated into the aphasia literature (e.g., [3, 710]). Most authors included in the temporal segment of the language area (Wernicke’s area) the first and sometimes also the second temporal gyrus (usually its posterior section) in general corresponding to Brodmann areas 21 and 22 (BA21 and BA22). Sometimes, the angular and the supramarginal gyri have also been included (e.g., [11]).

Recent research using contemporary neuroimaging techniques (e.g., PET and fMRI) has reanalyzed the exact localization of the language understanding area in the temporal lobe, attempting to pinpoint the functional anatomy of language (e.g., [1214]). Several proposals have been presented.

Ferstl et al. [15] conducted a meta-analysis of 23 neuroimaging studies on text comprehension directed to confirm the extension of the brain network involved in processing language in context. It was found that independent of the baseline the anterior temporal lobes were bilaterally active. In addition, processing of coherent compared with incoherent text engaged the left dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate cortex. Right hemisphere activations were seen most notably in the analysis of contrasts testing specific subprocesses, such as metaphor comprehension. These results suggest that when language comprehension is processed in a context, it is associated with an extensive brain activation network, involving not just the left, but also the right hemisphere.

Démonet et al. [16] used Positron Emission Technology (PET) to analyze brain activation during phonological and lexical semantic processing; they found the first one associated with activation of the left superior temporal gyrus, whereas the latter was linked to activity in the left middle and inferior temporal gyri. These findings are congruent with the aphasia literature, which demonstrates that the superior temporal gyrus damage results in phoneme discrimination defects, whereas lexical impairments are found in cases of middle temporal gyrus pathology, and semantic defects are usually observed in cases of posterior inferior temporal-occipital damage (e.g., [4, 17]).

DeWitt and Rauschecker [12] have proposed that “Wernicke’s area” may be better construed as two cortical modules, an auditory word-form area in the auditory ventral stream and an “inner speech area” in the auditory dorsal stream, thus emphasizing the heterogeneity of this language processing area. Dronkers et al. [13] pointed out that traditional language areas, such as Wernicke’s area, may serve somewhat different functions than originally described; they suggest that the analysis of more specific deficits and their anatomical correlates can lead to improved mapping of language functions in the brain.

Saygin et al. [18] assessed the relationship between verbal and nonverbal auditory processing by examining the ability of 30 left hemisphere-damaged aphasic patients to match environmental sounds and linguistic phrases to corresponding pictures. Lesion overlay analysis indicated that damage to posterior regions in the left middle and superior temporal gyri and to the inferior parietal lobe was a predictor of deficits in processing for both speech and environmental sounds. The lesion mapping revealed a posterior superior temporal region (Wernicke’s area) as being differentially more important for processing nonverbal sounds compared with verbal sounds, suggesting that language shares neural resources with those used for processing information in other domains. Congruent with this observation, Wise et al. [19] using PET identified two anatomically separable, functional subsystems in the left temporal cortex. One part, directed along the supratemporal cortical plane, responded to both nonspeech and speech sounds, including the sound of the speaker’s own voice. The second, more lateral and ventral part lay in the posterior left superior temporal sulcus, a region that responded to an external source of speech.

Noteworthy, departing from fMRI studies Binder et al. [20] refer to the existence of left hemisphere temporoparietal language areas outside the traditional “Wernicke area,” namely, in the middle temporal, inferior temporal, fusiform, and angular gyri; it means kind of “extended Wernicke’s area.” This implies that, in addition to classical Wernicke’s area (first and second temporal gyrus) involved in auditory language processing, there are also some adjacent brain areas, such as the inferior temporal gyrus and the angular gyrus, participating in language processing.

Different recent functional studies have suggested the involvement of BA20 (inferior temporal gyrus and anterior part of the fusiform gyrus) in language processing (see [21]), including lexicosemantic processing [22, 23], metaphor comprehension [24], language comprehension and production [25], and selective attention to speech [26]. BA20 atrophy, on the other hand, has been observed in semantic dementia [27], supporting its involvement in semantic language understanding.

In previous meta-analytic connectivity studies, we illustrated that the left fusiform gyrus (BA37), the left temporal pole (BA38), and the angular gyrus (BA39) were clearly involved in the language processing [2830]. In this paper, the potential participation of BA20 in language is analyzed. BA20 is bounded medially by BA36 (ectorhinal area), laterally by BA21 (middle temporal gyrus), rostrally by BA38 (temporal pole), and caudally by BA37 (posterior inferior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus). This analysis has the purpose of further pinpointing the temporal areas involved in language processing.

Currently, there are several techniques that can potentially demonstrate brain circuitries or networks. These techniques are grouped under the term “brain connectivity." Recently, a new alternative to study brain connectivity has been proposed by Robinson et al. [31] known as Meta-Analytic Connectivity Modeling or MACM. MACM is based on automatic meta-analysis done by pooling coactivation patterns. The technique takes advantage of the Brainmap.org’s repository of functional MRI studies and of a special software (Sleuth) provided by the same group, to find, filter, organize, plot, and export the peaks coordinates for further statistical analysis of its results. Sleuth provides a list of foci, in Talairach or MNI coordinates, each one representing the center of mass of a cluster of activation. The method takes the region of interest (e.g., BA20), makes it the independent variable, and interrogates the database for studies showing activation of the chosen target. The query is easily filtered with different conditions (such as age, normal versus patients, type of paradigm, and domain of cognition). By pooling the data with these conditions the tool provides a universe of coactivations that can be statistically analyzed for significant commonality. As a final step, Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) [32, 33] is performed utilizing GingerALE, another software also provided by Brainmap, assessing the probability of an event to occur at voxel level across the studies. Areas of coactivation will show a network related to the function and domains selected as filter criteria. It is assumed that if two or more areas are activated within the same task, they are interconnected and participate in a single network.

The present study was aimed at demonstrating the networks in which BA20 is involved during the performance of different language tasks using Meta-Analytic Connectivity Modeling (MACM). Simultaneous activation of two or more areas when performing a particular task indicates that those areas may be interconnected and consequently belong to a unified brain network. Coactivation however does not allow conclusion about the direction and sequence of activation.

2. Materials and Methods

The DataBase of Brainmap [21] was accessed utilizing Sleuth 2.2 on July 20, 2015. Sleuth is the software provided by Brainmap to query its database. The meta-analysis intends to assess the network of coactivations in which BA20 is involved.

The search conditions were as follows: (1) studies reporting BA20 activation; (2) using fMRI; (3) context: normal subjects; (4) activations: activation only; (5) handedness: right-handed subjects; (6) age 18–60 years; (7) domain: cognition, subtype: language; (8) language: English.

ALE meta-analysis was then performed utilizing GingerALE. ALE maps were thresholded at for multiple comparisons and false discovery rate. Only clusters of 200 or more cubic mm were accepted as valid clusters. ALE results were overlaid onto an anatomical template suitable for MNI coordinates, also provided by BrainMap.org. For this purpose we utilized the Multi-Image Analysis GUI (Mango) [34]. Mosaics of 5 × 6 insets of transversal fusion images were generated utilizing a plugin of the same tool, selecting every other image, starting on image number 10, and exported to a 2D-jpg image. A 3D rendition of the brain was also obtained; the left hemisphere lateral view has been chosen for display.

3. Results

Eleven papers corresponding to 12 experimental conditions with a total of 207 subjects were selected (subjects participating in two different experiments were counted as two subjects) (Table 1).

Table 1: Studies of language paradigms included in the meta-analysis (11 papers, 207 subjects, 12 experiments, and 174 foci).

Table 2 presents the main loci of brain connectivity of BA20 by Meta-Analytic Connectivity Modeling (MACM). Seven different clusters of activation were found, all related to the left hemisphere (Figure 1).

Table 2: Main loci of brain connectivity of BA20 in language tasks by Meta-Analytic Connectivity Modeling (MACM).
Figure 1: Functional connectivity map of BA20 by Meta-Analytic Connectivity Modeling. (a) Transversal descending cuts of the brain MRI template. Left hemisphere appears on the left side (neurological convention). Clusters of activation are color coded for statistical significance from dark blue (lowest) to red (highest). (b) 3D volumetric rendition of the brain showing activation on the left hemisphere surface. Red color zone identifies BA20. Deep and midline activations are shown.

Regardless of the BA20 activation during diverse linguistic tasks, the volumes of the clusters were small (see “volume” in Table 2 and Figure 1). The first cluster includes the left temporal lobe, BA20 and BA21, whereas Cluster #2 was located at the insula (BA13) and the prefrontal BA46. Cluster #3 involved the inferior frontal lobe (BA47) and Cluster #4 was situated in the left inferior temporal lobe (BA37). Cluster #5 was again situated in the left prefrontal cortex (BA9). The last two clusters involved the cingulate gyrus (BA30) (Cluster #6) and the occipital gyrus (BA19) (Cluster #7).

Indeed there were only few activated areas outside the temporal lobe: insula (BA13), prefrontal cortex (BA46 and BA9), cingulate gyrus (BA30), and the occipital lobe (BA19). Yet in all the cases the level of activation was modest; and all the clusters were situated in the left hemisphere.

4. Discussion

Regardless of the diverse limitations that can be pointed to the present study (specific characteristics of the sample, implicit limitations of the method that was used, inclusion of language as a whole without distinguishing different language abilities, modality of the stimuli presentation, considering subjects participating in two different experiments as two different subjects, etc.), current meta-analysis illustrates that BA20 has certain, albeit limited, participation in language processes. It is mainly connected with other areas of the temporal lobe, the insula, and the prefrontal cortex. Minor interconnections with the cingulate gyrus and the occipital lobe were also observed.

Retaking the initial question of this paper (“how extended Wernicke’s area is?”), our results suggest that, regardless of the fact that BA20 participates in language processes, in no way can it be considered as a core language processing area (Wernicke’s area). Hence, it could be interpreted as a kind of language processing marginal area. A similar secondary role in language processing has been suggested for other temporal (BA37 and BA38) [28, 29] and parietal (BA39) [30] areas.

To further pinpoint the extension of Wernicke’s area, the potential participation of two additional retrorolandic areas in language should be considered: supramarginal gyrus (BA40) and primary auditory cortex (BA41 and BA42).

The involvement of BA40 (supramarginal gyrus) in language processing is supported by diverse previous reports. Damage in BA40 relates to conduction aphasia [3537]; and contemporary neuroimaging studies support BA40 involvement in several language functions: attention to phonological relations [38], semantic processing [30], verbal creativity [39], writing [40], phonological processing and verbal working memory [41], and language production [42]. Some authors have even suggested that BA40 should be considered as a part of Wernicke’s area [11].

Damage in BA41 and BA42 (primary auditory cortex) on the other hand is associated with “pure word-deafness” (sometimes referred to as “auditory verbal agnosia”) [4348]; this unusual syndrome is characterized by an inability to understand spoken language with preserved speech production and reading ability [49]. It is usually regarded as a fragment of Wernicke’s aphasia [50], and hence it seems reasonable to include BA41 and BA42 as a part of Wernicke’s area.

In summary, current information suggests that two posterior language areas can be distinguished: core Wernicke’s area (language auditory processing area in the left hemisphere) including not only BA22 and BA21 (as usually accepted), but also BA41 and BA42; a “language associations area” roughly corresponding to BA20, BA37, BA38, BA39, and BA40 (“extended Wernicke’s area” or “Wernicke’s system”).

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interests in this paper.

Acknowledgment

The authors express their sincere gratitude to Adriana Ardila for her valuable editorial support.

References

  1. J. E. Bogen and G. M. Bogen, “Wernicke's region–where is it?” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 280, no. 1, pp. 834–843, 1976. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  2. A. Ardila, Aphasia Handbook, Florida International University, Miami, Fla, USA, 2014.
  3. D. F. Benson, Aphasia, Alexia and Agraphia, Churchill Livingstone, New York, NY, USA, 1979.
  4. A. R. Luria, Basic Problems in Neurolinguistics, Mouton, New York, NY, USA, 1976.
  5. C. Wernicke, “The aphasic symptom-complex: a psychological study on an anatomical basis,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 280–282, 1970. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  6. J. Dejerine, Semiologie des Affections du Systeme Nerveux, Masson, Paris, France, 1914.
  7. R. Brain, Speech Disorders Aphasia, Apraxia and Agnosia, Butterworth, London, UK, 1961.
  8. H. Head, Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech, Cambridge University Press, London, UK, 1926.
  9. J. M. Nielsen, Agnosia, Apraxia, Aphasia: Their Value in Cerebral Localization, Hoeber, New York, NY, USA, 1936.
  10. A. R. Luria, Traumatic Aphasia, Mouton, The Hague, The Netherlands, 1947/1970.
  11. M.-M. Mesulam, “From sensation to cognition,” Brain, vol. 121, no. 6, pp. 1013–1052, 1998. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  12. I. DeWitt and J. P. Rauschecker, “Wernicke's area revisited: parallel streams and word processing,” Brain and Language, vol. 127, no. 2, pp. 181–191, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  13. N. F. Dronkers, B. B. Redfern, and R. T. Knight, “The neural architecture of language disorders,” in The New Cognitive Neurosciences, M. S. Gazzaniga, Ed., pp. 949–958, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2000. View at Google Scholar
  14. D. Poeppel and G. Hickok, “Towards a new functional anatomy of language,” Cognition, vol. 92, no. 1-2, pp. 1–12, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  15. E. C. Ferstl, J. Neumann, C. Bogler, and D. Y. Von Cramon, “The extended language network: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 29, no. 5, pp. 581–593, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  16. J.-F. Démonet, F. Chollet, S. Ramsay et al., “The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects,” Brain, vol. 115, no. 6, pp. 1753–1768, 1992. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  17. D. F. Benson and A. Ardila, Aphasia: A Clinical Perspective, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1996.
  18. A. P. Saygin, F. Dick, S. W. Wilson, N. F. Dronkers, and E. Bates, “Neural resources for processing language and environmental sounds: evidence from aphasia,” Brain, vol. 126, no. 4, pp. 928–945, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  19. R. J. S. Wise, S. K. Scott, S. C. Blank, C. J. Mummery, K. Murphy, and E. A. Warburton, “Separate neural subsystems within ‘Wernicke's area’,” Brain, vol. 124, no. 1, pp. 83–95, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  20. J. R. Binder, J. A. Frost, T. A. Hammeke, R. W. Cox, S. M. Rao, and T. Prieto, “Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 353–362, 1997. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  21. http://brainmap.org/.
  22. A. Ischebeck, P. Indefrey, N. Usui, I. Nose, F. Hellwig, and M. Taira, “Reading in a regular orthography: an fMRI study investigating the role of visual familiarity,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 16, no. 5, pp. 727–741, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  23. R. Heun, F. Jessen, U. Klose, M. Erb, D.-O. Granath, and W. Grodd, “Response-related fMRI of veridical and false recognition of words,” European Psychiatry, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 42–52, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  24. A. M. Rapp, D. T. Leube, M. Erb, W. Grodd, and T. T. J. Kircher, “Neural correlates of metaphor processing,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 395–402, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  25. D. Papathanassiou, O. Etard, E. Mellet, L. Zago, B. Mazoyer, and N. Tzourio-Mazoyer, “A common language network for comprehension and production: a contribution to the definition of language epicenters with PET,” NeuroImage, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 347–357, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  26. V. A. Vorobyev, K. Alho, S. V. Medvedev et al., “Linguistic processing in visual and modality-nonspecific brain areas: PET recordings during selective attention,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 309–322, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  27. C. J. Mummery, K. Patterson, C. J. Price, J. Ashburner, R. S. J. Frackowiak, and J. R. Hodges, “A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 47, no. 1, pp. 36–45, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  28. A. Ardila, B. Bernal, and M. Rosselli, “The elusive role of the left temporal pole (BA38) in language: a preliminary meta-analytic connectivity study,” International Journal of Brain Science, vol. 2014, Article ID 946039, 7 pages, 2014. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  29. A. Ardila, B. Bernal, and M. Rosselli, “Language and visual perception associations: meta-analytic connectivity modeling of Brodmann area 37,” Behavioural Neurology, vol. 2015, Article ID 565871, 14 pages, 2015. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  30. M. Rosselli, A. Ardila, and B. Bernal, “Modelo de conectividad de la circunvolución angular en el lenguaje: meta-análisis de neuroimágenes funcionales,” Revista de Neurología, vol. 60, no. 11, pp. 495–503, 2015. View at Google Scholar
  31. J. L. Robinson, A. R. Laird, D. C. Glahn, W. R. Lovallo, and P. T. Fox, “Metaanalytic connectivity modeling: delineating the functional connectivity of the human amygdala,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 31, no. 2, pp. 173–184, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  32. A. R. Laird, P. M. Fox, C. J. Price et al., “ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 25, no. 1, pp. 155–164, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  33. P. E. Turkeltaub, G. F. Eden, K. M. Jones, and T. A. Zeffiro, “Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation,” NeuroImage, vol. 16, no. 3, part 1, pp. 765–780, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  34. http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/.
  35. D. F. Benson, W. A. Sheremata, R. Bouchard, J. M. Segarra, D. Price, and N. Geschwind, “Conduction aphasia: a clinicopathological study,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 28, no. 5, pp. 339–346, 1973. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  36. H. Damasio and A. R. Damasio, “The anatomical basis of conduction aphasia,” Brain, vol. 103, no. 2, pp. 337–350, 1980. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  37. H. Goodglass, “Diagnosis of conduction aphasia,” in Conduction Aphasia, pp. 39–49, The Aphasia Center, St. Petersburg, Fla, USA, 1992. View at Google Scholar
  38. K. B. McDermott, S. E. Petersen, J. M. Watson, and J. G. Ojemann, “A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 293–303, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  39. N. P. Bechtereva, A. D. Korotkov, S. V. Pakhomov, M. S. Roudas, M. G. Starchenko, and S. V. Medvedev, “PET study of brain maintenance of verbal creative activity,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 53, no. 1, pp. 11–20, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  40. I. Rektor, I. Rektorová, M. Mikl, M. Brázdil, and P. Krupa, “An event-related fMRI study of self-paced alphabetically ordered writing of single letters,” Experimental Brain Research, vol. 173, no. 1, pp. 79–85, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  41. I. Deschamps, S. R. Baum, and V. L. Gracco, “On the role of the supramarginal gyrus in phonological processing and verbal working memory: evidence from rTMS studies,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 53, no. 1, pp. 39–46, 2014. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  42. D. S. Margulies and M. Petrides, “Distinct parietal and temporal connectivity profiles of ventrolateral frontal areas involved in language production,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 33, no. 42, pp. 16846–16852, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  43. G. Ulrich, “Interhemispheric functional relationships in auditory agnosia: an analysis of the preconditions and a conceptual model,” Brain and Language, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 286–300, 1978. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  44. H. Ackermann and K. Mathiak, “Symptomatologie, pathologischanatomische Grundlaqen und Pathomechanismen zentraler Hörstörungen (reine Worttaubheit, auditive Agnosie, Rindentaubheit),” Fortschritte der Neurologie-Psychiatrie, vol. 67, no. 11, pp. 509–523, 1999. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  45. G. Denes and C. Semenza, “Auditory modality specific anomia: evidence from a case of pure word deafness,” Cortex, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 401–411, 1975. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  46. D. Poeppel, “Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code,” Cognitive Science, vol. 25, no. 5, pp. 679–693, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  47. N. Takahashi, M. Kawamura, H. Shinotou, K. Hirayama, K. Kaga, and M. Shindo, “Pure word deafness due to left hemisphere damage,” Cortex, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 295–303, 1992. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  48. S. C. Wolberg, J. A. Temlett, and V. U. Fritz, “Pure word deafness,” South African Medical Journal, vol. 78, no. 11, pp. 668–670, 1990. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  49. A. Kussmaul, “Disturbances of speech,” in Enyclopedia of the Practice of Medicine, H. von Ziemssen, Ed., pp. 581–875, William Wood, New York, NY, USA, 1877. View at Google Scholar
  50. S. H. Auerbach, T. Allard, M. Naeser, M. P. Alexander, and M. L. Albert, “Pure word deafness. Analysis of a case with bilateral lesions and a defect at the prephonemic level,” Brain, vol. 105, no. 2, pp. 271–300, 1982. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus