Parkinson’s Disease

Parkinson’s Disease / 2016 / Article

Research Article | Open Access

Volume 2016 |Article ID 8675930 | 9 pages | https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/8675930

Clinical Pain and Neuropsychological Functioning in Parkinson’s Disease: Are They Related?

Academic Editor: Jan O. Aasly
Received17 Sep 2015
Revised13 Dec 2015
Accepted16 Dec 2015
Published13 Jan 2016

Abstract

Introduction. Pain is an important nonmotor symptom of Parkinson’s disease (PD). Brain areas such as the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex play an important role in the processing of pain. Since these brain areas are also involved in cognitive functioning, for example, episodic memory and executive functions, respectively, we examined whether a relationship exists between cognitive functioning and spontaneous pain in PD. Methods. Forty-eight patients with PD and 57 controls participated. Cognitive functioning was measured by a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests. Both the sensory-discriminative aspect and the motivational-affective aspect of pain were assessed. Multiple linear regression analyses were performed to assess a relation between cognition and pain. Results. Cognition was related to neither the sensory nor the affective aspect of pain in our sample of PD patients. Variance in pain measures was primarily explained by symptoms of depression and anxiety. Discussion. The difference between the affective and the sensory aspect of pain might be due to the neuropathology of PD, which is mainly present in areas processing the affective aspect of pain. Pain treatment might improve when mood is taken into account. We provide several explanations for the lack of an association between pain and cognition.

1. Introduction

Nonmotor manifestations of Parkinson’s disease (PD) have received increasingly more attention over the past few decades, which is justified considering its impact on everyday life of PD patients [1]. Pain is one of these nonmotor manifestations, which has a large impact on quality of life [2, 3]. Pain is estimated to be present in two-thirds of PD patients [4] and has been reported already at the clinical onset of the disease [5, 6]. Additionally, a difference between PD patients and controls has been found in sensitivity to pain: both the tolerance and threshold of pain appear lowered in PD [7]. Thus, painful stimuli might be more painful for PD patients than for controls. The importance for everyday clinical practice is further emphasized when the possibility of an undertreatment of pain in this group was suggested by a study from Beiske and colleagues, who found that only half of the PD patients with pain received analgesics or physiotherapy [8].

Pain is a complex psychological and neurophysiological phenomenon and is processed in a neural network, involving the lateral and the medial pain system [9]. The lateral pain system is mainly involved in processing sensory-discriminative aspect (e.g., localization, intensity) of pain. It consists of the spinothalamic tract, which passes through the lateral thalamus and projects mainly towards sensory cortical areas. The medial pain system mainly processes motivational-affective and cognitive-evaluative aspects of pain (e.g., unpleasantness, suffering) and projects through medial thalamic nuclei to cognition- and emotion-related areas, for example, the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). For a detailed overview of these pain systems, the reader is referred elsewhere [1014]. The medial pain system appears to be primarily affected by the neuropathology of PD [15].

Pain in PD has been linked to various factors, such as dopaminergic fluctuations [16, 17], depressive symptoms [18], duration of disease [7], motor problems [19], and cognitive functioning [15]. Impairment of cognitive functioning, in particular, executive functioning (EF) and memory, is common in PD patients [20, 21]. In the general population, the relation between cognition and pain has been assessed before. Cognitive functions, such as executive functioning and attention, are thought to be inversely related to pain, implying that less pain coincides with better cognitive functioning. Performance on a cognitive test was related to acutely induced experimental pain in a study of Seminowicz and Davis [22]. Processing of pain was found to show significant overlap with an attention-specific network, taking away resources for cognitive performance: pain demands attention [22]. More complex relations have been suggested as well. For example, the degree to which cognitive performance is affected might depend on the difficulty of the task and the intensity of pain [2325]. In a study from Pickering and colleagues it was shown that the interaction between cognition and pain is not restricted to a concurrent cognitive task: a higher score on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, measured off-line) correlated with a higher pain tolerance (assessed by psychophysical mechanical and thermal techniques) in healthy elderly. The authors explain this relation by the ability to centrally integrate the multiple facets of pain [26]. The fact that there is a convincing overlap in areas involved in pain processing and areas involved in cognitive performance emphasizes the association between these functions.

Many of the pain processing areas eventually become affected in PD [27, 28]. Brainstem areas involved in suppression of pain, such as the periaqueductal grey and locus coeruleus, are affected already in an early stage of the disease. This might result in an increase of pain in PD patients, but when neocortical areas are affected in later stages of the disease, pain might decrease [15]. In other words, pain might subside when cognitive impairment progresses in PD. However, if a higher level of pain does remain and cognitive impairment worsens, patients might not be able to communicate their pain. This would hinder successful treatment. Therefore, the goal of the present study is to examine the possible relationship between cognitive functioning and pain in PD patients.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants
2.1.1. Inclusion Criteria

Patients were included when they had a diagnosis of PD according to the UK PD Society Brain Bank diagnostic criteria [29]. This diagnosis was confirmed by a neurologist. Furthermore, the subjects had no history of cerebral traumata or of psychiatric disorders unrelated to PD (e.g., schizophrenic episodes, major depressive disorder long before the onset of PD) and were able to understand the neuropsychological tests (i.e., MMSE score > 16).

2.1.2. Subject Characteristics

Forty-eight PD patients and 57 controls participated in this study. The patient group consisted of more males (40% females) than the control group (67% females) (, ). Patients ( years, ) were older (, ) than controls ( years, ).

2.1.3. Education

Education was measured on a 5-point rating scale (1 = primary school, unfinished; 2 = primary school; 3 = secondary school; 4 = higher secondary school; 5 = higher vocational training or university degree). Level of education was matched between groups (Mann-Whitney, , ).

2.1.4. Comorbidities

Comorbidities were recorded. As can be seen in Table 1, patients were significantly more often diagnosed with diabetes.


ComorbidityControlsParkinson’s diseaseDifference between groups
Percentage (number)Percentage (number)Chi-square value

Cardiac failure28.1% (16)29.2% (14)0.0150.901
Kidney disease1.8% (1)0% (0)0.8500.356
Lung disease10.5% (6)4.2% (2)1.4970.221
Diabetes5.3% (3)18.8% (9)4.6820.030
Hypertension29.8% (17)27.1% (13)0.0960.757
Peripheral arterial disease8.8% (5)14.6% (7)0.8690.351
Arthritis12.3% (7)8.9% (4)0.3010.583

2.1.5. Medical Ethical Committee

This study was approved by the medical ethical committee of the VU medical center. All participants received information about the study before signing an informed consent form.

3. Material and Procedure

3.1. Pain

Pain was assessed by visual analogue scales, a Faces Pain Scale, and a verbal descriptor scale.

Visual analogue scales: the Colored Analogue Scale (CAS) is a colored version of the visual analogue scale. To test pain intensity, the CAS Intensity was used. Here, subjects indicate how much pain they have on a scale ranging from “no pain” (light pink) to “maximal pain” (dark red). To test suffering from pain, the CAS Affect was used. The CAS Affect is a similar color-coded scale ranging from “no suffering” to “maximal suffering.” Both versions of the CAS range from 0 to 100.

The Faces Pain Scale (FPS) primarily measures pain intensity, but also pain affect [30]. The FPS consists of seven pictures of faces, with a pain expression ranging from “no pain” to “most severe pain.” Subjects choose a face that represents their pain best. Answer possibilities of the FPS range from 0 to 6.

Verbal descriptor scale: the Number of Words Chosen-Affective (NWC-A) is the affective part of the McGill Pain Questionnaire and was utilized to investigate the affective aspect of pain [31, 32]. Three adjectives, all expressing an increasing amount of pain, are read aloud by the examiner. Subjects have to indicate which of the three adjectives, if any, best represents their pain. Possible scores range from 0 to 15.

3.2. Cognition
3.2.1. Memory

To assess memory, the following neuropsychological tests were administered.

The Eight-Word Test is part of the Amsterdam Dementia Screening test [33] and consists of three verbal memory subtests: immediate recall, delayed recall, and delayed recognition. In the immediate recall subtest, eight words are read aloud five times by the examiner and each time the participant is required to recall as many words from the list as possible. Maximum score for this subtest is 40. The delayed recall subtest is assessed after approximately 15 minutes. The participant is required to reproduce as many words from the original set of words as possible. Maximum score for this test is 8. The delayed recognition subtest is assessed immediately after the delayed recall test. The participant is required to recognize the initial set of eight words from a set of 16 words and indicate which words are new. Maximum for this test is 16.

Face Recognition is a subtest of the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT) and measures visual recognition memory [34]. Ten pictures of faces are presented to the participant in a fixed order. Approximately three minutes later the participant is asked to recognize the initial set of 10 faces from a set of 20 faces and indicate which faces are new. Maximum score for this test is 20.

Picture Recognition is another subtest of the RBMT and measures visual recognition memory. Twenty drawings of objects and animals are displayed to the participant in a fixed order. Approximately three minutes later the participant is asked to recognize the initial set of 20 drawings from a set of 40 drawings and indicate which drawings are new. For both RBMT subtests, number of correct answers minus number of incorrect answers was taken as an outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 40.

The Knox Cube Test was used to assess visual attention and memory [35]. Subjects are asked to imitate the pattern tapped by the researcher on a wooden model of four small, aligned blocks. The length of the tapped pattern increases, until the subject is unable to reproduce it. The number of correct test trials was recorded as outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 15.

3.2.2. Executive Functioning

To assess executive functioning, the following neuropsychological tests were administered.

The Digit Span Backward test from the Wechsler Memory Test was used to assess working memory [36]. Subjects are instructed to repeat a string of verbally presented digits backwards. The total amount of correct responses was used as the outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 21.

The Key Search test of the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) measures planning, monitoring, and regulation of behavior [34]. The subject is required to draw the route they would take in the hypothetical situation of trying to find a lost key on a large field. The drawn route is scored according to a predefined set of rules. This score was used as outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 16.

The Rule Shift test of the BADS measures cognitive flexibility and attention [37]. A set of playing cards is shown to the subject. In the first part, a response pattern is established according to a simple rule (i.e., “Yes” for a red card, “No” for a black card). For the second part, subjects are required to respond differently to the same set of cards. The outcome measure was the number of correct answers. Maximum score for this test is 19.

Category Fluency measures verbal ability and executive control: the subject is required to name as many animals within 60 seconds and as many professions in another trial of 60 seconds [38]. Total number of mentioned instances served as outcome measure.

The Picture Completion test of the Groninger Intelligence Test (GIT) was used to assess visual perception and alertness to detail [39]. Subjects are asked to complete a partly drawn figure, each with increasing difficulty (i.e., less complete). Number of correct answers was taken as outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 20.

3.2.3. Attention

To assess attention, the Digit Span Forward test from the Wechsler Memory Test was administered [36]. Subjects are instructed to repeat a string of verbally presented digits. Number of digits per string was increased until the subject was unable to reproduce the string. The total amount of correct responses was used as the outcome measure. Maximum score for this test is 21.

3.3. Mood

Depressive symptoms were evaluated with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [40]. The administered BDI consists of 20 questions, all addressing depressive symptoms, with answer options ranging from no symptoms (0) to severe symptoms (3).

Additionally, the depression and anxiety subscales of the Symptom Check List-90 were administered [41]. The subtest Depression consists of 15 symptoms, with answer options ranging from 1 (“not bothered at all”) to 5 (“extremely bothered”); the subtest Anxiety addresses 10 symptoms with the same answer options.

3.4. Procedure

Subjects were recruited through outpatient clinics of hospitals, patient societies, and immediate surroundings. Patients and controls were assessed in their own home and were given the freedom to choose the day and time of assessment. By this, we hope to overcome the possibility of suboptimal performance. The order of neuropsychological tests, pain assessments, and mood questionnaires was as follows: MMSE, CAS, FPS and NWC-A, Eight-Word Test-immediate recall condition, BADS rule-shifting subtest, BADS key search subtest, Eight-Word test-long-term recall and recognition condition, Digit Span Forward, Digit Span Backward, RBMT Faces (present faces), Fluency-animals, RBMT Faces-recognition condition, RBMT Pictures-presentation, Fluency-professions, RBMT Pictures-recognition condition, KNOX cubes, figure completion test, BDI, and SCL90 subscales anxiety and depression. Approximate duration of assessment was 1.5–2 hours.

3.5. Statistical Analyses

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 21, and Lavaan package in R were utilized to perform statistical analyses [42]. In order to determine whether PD patients and controls differed on pain measures, an Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was conducted with pain as dependent variable, group and gender as factors, and age as covariate. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to determine domains in cognitive scores. Four linear multiple regression analyses were performed within the PD group, all with one pain measure as outcome variable and memory, executive functioning, and attention performance and mood and gender as predictors. The alpha level was set at 0.05.

3.6. Cognitive Domains

A factor analysis was performed to reduce the number of predictors in the analysis. The 12 cognitive subtests were entered into a CFA analysis. Two domains were created: the domain Memory entailed three subtests of the Eight-Word Test (immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition), RBMT Face and Picture Recognition, KNOX, and Picture Completion. The domain executive functioning was represented by BADS Rule Shift test, BADS Key Search test, Digit Span Backward, and Category Fluency. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI) of the CFA was 0.912, and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) was 0.097 (), indicating that domains were indeed measuring the same construct. Digit Span Forward (DSF) served as an individual variable representing attention.

4. Results

4.1. Differences in Cognitive Functioning between PD Patients and Controls

The average MMSE score for patients (, ) was significantly lower than for control subjects (, , Mann-Whitney , , ). Patients performed worse on all neuropsychological subtests (see Table 2 for details).


ControlsParkinson’s diseaseMann-Whitney test () value
Mean (SD)RangeMean (SD)Range

MMSE28.00 (2.09)21–3026.14 (3.57)16–30−3.076 0.002
Eight-Word Test: immediate recall subtest32.23 (5.00)20–4025.86 (7.94)4–38−4.542<0.001
Eight-Word Test: delayed recall subtest5.81 (1.55)1–84.16 (2.37)0–8−3.8040.001
Eight-Word Test: delayed recognition subtest15.37 (0.86)12–1614.14 (2.30)6–16−3.3980.001
Rule Shift test (BADS)17.16 (3.05)9–1914.75 (4.17)6–19−3.3110.001
Key Search test (BADS)10.61 (4.07)3–168.55 (4.33)2–16−2.8920.004
Digit Span Forward13.28 (2.96)8–2111.84 (2.96)5–19−2.4960.013
Digit Span Backward9.00 (3.05)3–187.34 (2.69)3–14−3.0180.003
Face Recognition (RBMT)16.88 (3.14)6–2014.41 (4.50)2–20−3.1190.002
Picture Recognition (RBMT)38.58 (2.57)26–4036.55 (5.90)12–40−2.0550.040
Category Fluency39.30 (11.85)6–6730.48 (13.05)5–70−4.107<0.001
Knox Cube Test11.14 (1.70)7–1510.14 (1.91)5–14−2.8830.004
Picture Completion test (GIT)11.51 (3.36)5–189.75 (3.98)2–19−2.6350.008

4.2. Differences in Pain between PD Patients and Controls

After controlling for age, there was no main effect of group on CAS Intensity (; ).

There was a significant main effect of group on CAS Affect (; , partial ). Additionally, the effect of gender as a factor showed a trend on CAS Affect, with women experiencing more pain then men (; , partial ), and there was no significant interaction between these variables (; , partial ).

Patients showed to experience more pain as measures on the FPS than controls after controlling for age (; , partial ). Here, the effect of gender as a factor showed a trend (; , partial ), with women scoring higher on the FPS but without a significant interaction between these variables (; , partial ).

There was a main effect of group on NWC-A (; , partial ), as well as an effect of gender as a factor (; , partial ). There was no significant interaction between group and gender (; , partial ). See Table 3 for scores on the pain scales.


PD patientsControlsMain effect of group
Mean (SD)RangeMean (SD)Range-value (df) value (partial )

Colored Analogue Scale (CAS) Intensity25.74 (25.45)0–9530.88 (27.13)0–921.905 (1, 100)0.171 (0.019)
Faces Pain Scale (FPS)1.49 (1.31)0–52.17 (1.81)0–76.191 (1, 100)0.014 (0.058)
Colored Analogue Scale (CAS) Affect20.16 (24.95)0–7532.04 (27.27)0–906.803 (1, 100)0.010 (0.064)
Number of Words Chosen-Affective (NWC-A)2.02 (2.66)0–104.13 (3.64)0–1315.503 (1, 100)0.000 (0.134)

4.3. Linear Regression Analyses to Assess the Relationship between Pain and Cognition

Four multiple linear regression analyses were calculated to predict pain (as measured by the four different pain measures) based on Memory, EF, attention, gender, and Mood for the group of PD patients.

No significant regression equation was found for the multiple linear regression to predict outcome on CAS Intensity (, ), with an of 0.158.

The regression model for predicting scores on FPS significantly accounted for approximately 46% of the variance (, , ). However, none of the cognitive measures significantly contributed to the model. Mood and (male) gender showed a trend in predicting pain as measured by FPS.

The regression model for CAS Affect accounted for 25.7% of the variance (, ). As shown in Table 4, none of the cognitive measures contributed to the model. In this model, only mood significantly predicted scores on CAS Affect.


(1) Colored Analogue Scale (CAS) Intensity(2) Faces Pain Scale (FPS)(3) Colored Analogue Scale (CAS) Affect(4) Number of Word Chosen-Affective (NWC-A)

Memory5.9367.1010.186−0.6430.411−0.316−3.8606.608−0.122−0.9680.761−0.226
Executive functioning−5.7738.154−0.1650.3480.4720.1563.6077.5880.1060.4550.8740.097
Mood10.0505.0990.3440.5290.2950.28413.0844.7460.4512.1980.5470.560
Attention−0.4224.963−0.0160.1340.2880.0780.0884.6190.0030.2960.5320.082
Gender−4.6928.475−0.089−0.9970.491−0.298−5.2300.888−0.101−1.1030909−0.157

Similar results were found for the regression model predicting scores on the NWC-A (, , ): none of the cognitive measures were contributing to the model. Mood was the only significant predictor in this model. See Table 4 for details.

5. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to address the association between cognition and pain in PD patients. Although pain intensity did not significantly differ between groups consistently (CAS Intensity, FPS), PD patients experienced more pain effect (CAS Affect, NWC-A).

When compared to controls, it has been found that PD patients experience pain more frequently [4]. However, this has not been found consistently: others (e.g., Quittenbaum and Grahn) report that pain in PD does not differ from pain in the general population [3]. Even though Quittenbaum and Grahn did not include an assessment of all aspects of pain, we propose that this inconsistency in findings might be explained by a difference between the aspects of pain: in our sample, patients may have suffered more from pain than controls, whereas the sensory-discriminative aspect was comparable to controls. This discrepancy might be explained by the progression of PD-neuropathology throughout the pain systems: the lateral pain system (sensory-discriminative aspect) remains relatively intact, whereas the medial pain system, involved in the affective aspect of pain, shows more neuropathology [15, 27].

The main goal of this study was to examine a possible relationship between pain and cognition in PD. We hypothesized that this relationship could be either positive or negative and that specifically a negative relationship would be clinically relevant, considering the risk of undertreating pain in cognitively impaired patients who might have more difficulty communicating their pain to a physician. In contrast to our hypothesis, cognitive functioning showed no relationship with pain in our sample of PD patients. Yet similar to other studies, symptoms of mood disorders strongly influenced pain experience. In healthy elderly, a positive relationship between MMSE scores and pain tolerance has been found previously [26]. The relationship between everyday clinical pain and cognitive performance has been researched before, albeit not in Parkinson’s disease: for a group of Alzheimer’s disease patients, a positive correlation between executive functioning and pain was found. However, the authors emphasized the need to control for symptoms of mood disorders when studying the relation between pain and cognition [43]. This was confirmed in a study looking into patients with rheumatoid arthritis, where cognition was negatively correlated to pain [44]. This correlation disappeared when the authors additionally assessed the mediating effect of depression, indicating the major influence of depression on both pain and cognition.

One possible explanation for the lack of a significant association between pain and cognitive functioning might be that neural functional reorganization takes place to compensate for deficits on cognitive performance. These compensatory processes may result in the activation of other brain regions during tasks that measure memory and executive functioning than in controls. These brain regions may not be as involved in the processing of pain as they are in the controls. An example of this shift in recruited brain regions is found in multiple sclerosis (MS). In a study investigating MS patients, increased and additional activation of frontal and posterior parietal cortices was observed during several attention tasks. These areas were not or only partly activated in controls, during the same tasks [45, 46]. In PD, a recent study investigated functional connectivity and cognitive functioning in PD patients [47]. The authors concluded that, in the beginning of the disease, a state of hyperconnectivity arises and that a hypoconnectivity is associated with more cognitively impaired patients. They also state that this might be a compensatory mechanism of the brain. A similar mechanism might apply for pain: a decrease in gray matter in pain areas (e.g., ACC, insula) has been found for several chronic pain disorders and could even be the consequence of chronic pain [48]. A deviant neural functioning as a consequence of the disease (the compensatory neural mechanism) or of the symptoms themselves (pain) might therefore explain the lack of association between pain and cognition.

Our study confirms that mood disorders are the main contributor to pain experience in PD: symptoms of anxiety and depression were predictive for three out of four pain parameters. This finding is in line with previous research [49]. The relation between pain and mood could be bidirectional: symptoms of depression or anxiety might increase pain experience, and, vice versa, pain might exacerbate symptoms of depression and anxiety [49]. In our sample, NWC-A and CAS Affect showed the strongest relationship with mood. The FPS and CAS Intensity, both focusing on intensity (sensory-discriminative aspect) of pain, showed a weaker association [50]. This is an interesting finding, since it strengthens our belief that pain experience consists of several components, all of which should be paid equal attention. Clinically, it also indicates that pain treatment will probably benefit when mood is taken into account.

5.1. Limitations

Several limitations apply to this study. Firstly, regarding cognitive functioning, no patients showed severe cognitive impairment, which reduces the variability in the sample. Subsequently, our hypothesized relationship might not be as pronounced as it would have been when this increased variability would be present in our sample. We chose to exclude patients with a severe cognitive impairment because they are no longer able to fully understand the neuropsychological tests. However, this unfortunately results in a reduction of variability of cognitive functioning. Secondly, use of pain medication was not recorded. Analgesic medication (evidently) influences pain experience and therefore might also influence the relationship between cognition and pain. In addition, participants were not required to withhold dopaminergic medication. This might influence our results in two ways. Indirectly, the reduction of (painful) motor symptoms during the “on” period might bring pain relief. Directly, dopaminergic medication has an ameliorating influence on pain experience as it has been shown that hypofunctioning of nigrostriatal dopamine is associated with an increase in pain experience in healthy persons [51]. Indeed, PD patients show a lowered pain threshold during their “off” period, and the pain threshold returns to a normal level after levodopa administration [52], although this effect has also been found to differ depending on how the patient responds to dopaminergic medication [53]. The dopaminergic antinociceptive effect might therefore also have an influence on the association between pain and cognition in our study. Finally, several clinical and demographical variables have not been taken into account, such as disease duration and severity of the disease. These have been shown to influence pain experience [54]. Regarding the comorbidities, there were more patients with diabetes as comorbidity than controls, which poses the possibility of, for example, encephalopathy.

6. Conclusion

This study suggests that symptoms of mood disorders are an important predictor for the experience of pain in PD patients, probably of more significance than cognitive dysfunction in these patients, since we did not find a significant relationship between pain experience and cognition.

Conflict of Interests

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interests regarding the publication of this paper.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for the support received by Stichting ParkinsonFonds.

References

  1. P. Martinez-Martin, C. Rodriguez-Blazquez, M. M. Kurtis, and K. R. Chaudhuri, “The impact of non-motor symptoms on health-related quality of life of patients with Parkinson's disease,” Movement Disorders, vol. 26, no. 3, pp. 399–406, 2011. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  2. D. A. Gallagher, A. J. Lees, and A. Schrag, “What are the most important nonmotor symptoms in patients with Parkinson's disease and are we missing them?” Movement Disorders, vol. 25, no. 15, pp. 2493–2500, 2010. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  3. B. H. Quittenbaum and B. Grahn, “Quality of life and pain in Parkinson's disease: a controlled cross-sectional study,” Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 129–136, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  4. M. P. G. Broen, M. M. Braaksma, J. Patijn, and W. E. J. Weber, “Prevalence of pain in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review using the modified QUADAS tool,” Movement Disorders, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 480–484, 2012. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  5. G. Defazio, A. Berardelli, G. Fabbrini et al., “Pain as a nonmotor symptom of Parkinson disease: evidence from a case-control study,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 65, no. 9, pp. 1191–1194, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  6. L. Nègre-Pagès, W. Regragui, D. Bouhassira, H. Grandjean, and O. Rascol, “Chronic pain in Parkinson's disease: the cross-sectional French DoPaMiP survey,” Movement Disorders, vol. 23, no. 10, pp. 1361–1369, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  7. S. Z. Marsala, M. Tinazzi, R. Vitaliani et al., “Spontaneous pain, pain threshold, and pain tolerance in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Neurology, vol. 258, no. 4, pp. 627–633, 2011. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  8. A. G. Beiske, J. H. Loge, A. Rønningen, and E. Svensson, “Pain in Parkinson's disease: prevalence and characteristics,” Pain, vol. 141, no. 1-2, pp. 173–177, 2009. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  9. B. Kulkarni, D. E. Bentley, R. Elliott et al., “Attention to pain localization and unpleasantness discriminates the functions of the medial and lateral pain systems,” European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 21, no. 11, pp. 3133–3142, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  10. E. J. A. Scherder, J. A. Sergeant, and D. F. Swaab, “Pain processing in dementia and its relation to neuropathology,” The Lancet Neurology, vol. 2, no. 11, pp. 677–686, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  11. P. Rainville, “Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 195–204, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  12. A. V. Apkarian, M. C. Bushnell, R.-D. Treede, and J.-K. Zubieta, “Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease,” European Journal of Pain, vol. 9, no. 4, pp. 463–484, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  13. R. Peyron, B. Laurent, and L. García-Larrea, “Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000),” Neurophysiologie Clinique, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 263–288, 2000. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  14. W. D. Willis and K. N. Westlund, “Neuroanatomy of the pain system and of the pathways that modulate pain,” Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 2–31, 1997. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  15. E. Scherder, E. Wolters, C. Polman, J. Sergeant, and D. Swaab, “Pain in Parkinson's disease and multiple sclerosis: its relation to the medial and lateral pain systems,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 29, no. 7, pp. 1047–1056, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  16. K. R. Chaudhuri and A. H. Schapira, “Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson's disease: dopaminergic pathophysiology and treatment,” The Lancet Neurology, vol. 8, no. 5, pp. 464–474, 2009. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  17. J. M. Jarcho, E. A. Mayer, Z. K. Jiang, N. A. Feier, and E. D. London, “Pain, affective symptoms, and cognitive deficits in patients with cerebral dopamine dysfunction,” Pain, vol. 153, no. 4, pp. 744–754, 2012. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  18. U. Ehrt, J. P. Larsen, and D. Aarsland, “Pain and its relationship to depression in parkinson disease,” The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 269–275, 2009. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  19. M. Tinazzi, C. Del Vesco, E. Fincati et al., “Pain and motor complications in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, vol. 77, no. 7, pp. 822–825, 2006. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  20. D. Aarsland, K. Bronnick, C. Williams-Gray et al., “Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. A multicenter pooled analysis,” Neurology, vol. 75, no. 12, pp. 1062–1069, 2010. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  21. D. Muslimović, B. Schmand, J. D. Speelman, and R. J. De Haan, “Course of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 13, no. 6, pp. 920–932, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  22. D. A. Seminowicz and K. D. Davis, “Pain enhances functional connectivity of a brain network evoked by performance of a cognitive task,” Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 97, no. 5, pp. 3651–3659, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  23. D. A. Seminowicz and K. D. Davis, “Interactions of pain intensity and cognitive load: the brain stays on task,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 1412–1422, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  24. J. Buhle and T. D. Wager, “Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task,” Pain, vol. 149, no. 1, pp. 19–26, 2010. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  25. D. A. Seminowicz and K. D. Davis, “A re-examination of pain–cognition interactions: implications for neuroimaging.,” Pain, vol. 130, no. 1-2, pp. 8–13, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  26. G. Pickering, D. Jourdan, A. Eschalier, and C. Dubray, “Impact of age, gender and cognitive functioning on pain perception,” Gerontology, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 112–118, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  27. H. Braak, K. Del Tredici, U. Rüb, R. A. I. De Vos, E. N. H. J. Steur, and E. Braak, “Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 197–211, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  28. K. Del Tredici, U. Rüb, R. A. I. De Vos, J. R. E. Bohl, and H. Braak, “Where does Parkinson disease pathology begin in the brain?” Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology, vol. 61, no. 5, pp. 413–426, 2002. View at: Google Scholar
  29. A. J. Hughes, S. E. Daniel, L. Kilford, and A. J. Lees, “Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, vol. 55, no. 3, pp. 181–184, 1992. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  30. D. Bieri, R. A. Reeve, G. D. Champion, L. Addicoat, and J. B. Ziegler, “The Faces Pain Scale for the self-assessment of the severity of pain experienced by children: development, initial validation, and preliminary investigation for ratio scale properties,” Pain, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 139–150, 1990. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  31. R. Melzack, “The McGill Pain Questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods,” Pain, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 277–299, 1975. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  32. R. J. Verkes, K. Vanderiet, H. Vertommen, W. A. Van der Kloot, and J. van der Meij, De MPQ-DLV. Een standaard nederlandstalige versie van de McGill Pain Questionnaire, Swets & Zeitlinger, 1989.
  33. J. Lindeboom and C. Jonker, Amsterdamse Dementie-Screeningtest, Handleiding, Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse, The Netherlands, 1989.
  34. B. A. Wilson, E. Greenfield, L. Clare et al., The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test—Extended Version, 1999.
  35. R. A. Bornstein, “Construct validity of the knox cube test as a neuropsychological measure,” Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 5, no. 2, pp. 105–114, 1983. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  36. D. Wechsler, “A standardized memory scale for clinical use,” The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 87–95, 1945. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  37. G. Norris and R. L. Tate, “The Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): ecological, concurrent and construct validity,” Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 33–45, 2000. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  38. Z. Shao, E. Janse, K. Visser, and A. S. Meyer, “What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 5, article 772, 2014. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  39. F. Luteijn and F. A. E. Van der Ploeg, Groninger Intelligentie Test, Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse, The Netherlands, 1983.
  40. A. Beck, C. Ward, and M. Mendelson, “Beck depression inventory (BDI),” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 4, pp. 561–571, 1961. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  41. L. R. Derogatis, K. Rickels, and A. F. Rock, “The SCL 90 and the MMPI: a step in the validation of a new self report scale,” The British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 128, no. 3, pp. 280–289, 1976. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  42. Y. Rosseel, “Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling,” Journal of Statistical Software, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 1–36, 2012. View at: Google Scholar
  43. E. J. A. Scherder, L. Eggermont, B. Plooij et al., “Relationship between chronic pain and cognition in cognitively intact older persons and in patients with Alzheimer's disease: the need to control for mood,” Gerontology, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 50–58, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  44. S. C. Brown, J. M. Glass, and D. C. Park, “The relationship of pain and depression to cognitive function in rheumatoid arthritis patients,” Pain, vol. 96, no. 3, pp. 279–284, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  45. I.-K. Penner, K. Opwis, and L. Kappos, “Relation between functional brain imaging, cognitive impairment and cognitive rehabilitation in patients with multiple sclerosis,” Journal of Neurology, vol. 254, no. 2, pp. II53–II57, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  46. I.-K. Penner, M. Rausch, L. Kappos, K. Opwis, and E. W. Radü, “Analysis of impairment related functional architecture in MS patients during performance of different attention tasks,” Journal of Neurology, vol. 250, no. 4, pp. 461–472, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  47. M. Gorges, H.-P. Müller, D. Lulé, E. H. Pinkhardt, A. C. Ludolph, and J. Kassubek, “To rise and to fall: functional connectivity in cognitively normal and cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson's disease,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 1727–1735, 2015. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  48. A. May, “Chronic pain may change the structure of the brain,” Pain, vol. 137, no. 1, pp. 7–15, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  49. M. J. Bair, R. L. Robinson, W. Katon, and K. Kroenke, “Depression and pain comorbidity: a literature review,” Archives of Internal Medicine, vol. 163, no. 20, pp. 2433–2445, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  50. D. A. Perrott, B. Goodenough, and G. D. Champion, “Children's ratings of the intensity and unpleasantness of post-operative pain using facial expression scales,” European Journal of Pain, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 119–127, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  51. N. Hagelberg, S. K. Jääskeläinen, I. K. Martikainen et al., “Striatal dopamine D2 receptors in modulation of pain in humans: a review,” European Journal of Pharmacology, vol. 500, no. 1–3, pp. 187–192, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  52. C. Brefel-Courbon, P. Payoux, C. Thalamas et al., “Effects of levodopa on pain threshold in Parkinson's disease: a clinical and positron emission tomography study,” Movement Disorders, vol. 20, no. 12, pp. 1557–1563, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  53. S.-Y. Lim, M. J. Farrell, S. J. Gibson, R. D. Helme, A. E. Lang, and A. H. Evans, “Do dyskinesia and pain share common pathophysiological mechanisms in Parkinson's disease?” Movement Disorders, vol. 23, no. 12, pp. 1689–1695, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  54. P. Valkovic, M. Minar, H. Singliarova et al., “Pain in Parkinson's disease: a cross-sectional study of its prevalence, types, and relationship to depression and quality of life,” PLoS ONE, vol. 10, no. 8, Article ID e0136541, 2015. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar

Copyright © 2016 Gwenda Engels et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1052 Views | 545 Downloads | 1 Citation
 PDF  Download Citation  Citation
 Download other formatsMore
 Order printed copiesOrder

We are committed to sharing findings related to COVID-19 as quickly and safely as possible. Any author submitting a COVID-19 paper should notify us at help@hindawi.com to ensure their research is fast-tracked and made available on a preprint server as soon as possible. We will be providing unlimited waivers of publication charges for accepted articles related to COVID-19. Sign up here as a reviewer to help fast-track new submissions.