Table of Contents Author Guidelines Submit a Manuscript
Autism Research and Treatment
Volume 2013, Article ID 963697, 8 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/963697
Review Article

Early Infant Exposure to Excess Multivitamin: A Risk Factor for Autism?

1Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Medical College, Dalian University, Dalian 116622, China
2Section of Cell Signaling, Okazaki Institute for Integrative Bioscience, National Institutes of Natural Sciences, Okazaki 444-8787, Japan
3Department of Physiology, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, China Medical University, Shenyang 110001, China
4Department of Neurology, Affiliated Zhongshan Hospital of Dalian University, Dalian 116001, China

Received 13 November 2012; Accepted 12 February 2013

Academic Editor: Herbert Roeyers

Copyright © 2013 Shi-Sheng Zhou et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Autism, a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects boys more than girls, is often associated with altered levels of monoamines (serotonin and catecholamines), especially elevated serotonin levels. The monoamines act as both neurotransmitters and signaling molecules in the gastrointestinal and immune systems. The evidence related to monoamine metabolism may be summarized as follows: (i) monoamine neurotransmitters are enzymatically degraded/inactivated by three mechanisms: oxidative deamination, methylation, and sulfation. The latter two are limited by the supply of methyl groups and sulfate, respectively. (ii) A decrease in methylation- and sulfation-mediated monoamine inactivation can be compensated by an increase in the oxidative deamination catalyzed by monoamine oxidase, an X-linked enzyme exhibiting higher activity in females than in males. (iii) Vitamins can, on one hand, facilitate the synthesis of monoamine neurotransmitters and, on the other hand, inhibit their inactivation by competing for methylation and sulfation. Therefore, we postulate that excess multivitamin feeding in early infancy, which has become very popular over the past few decades, may be a potential risk factor for disturbed monoamine metabolism. In this paper, we will focus on the relationship between excess multivitamin exposure and the inactivation/degradation of monoamine neurotransmitters and its possible role in the development of autism.

1. Introduction

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder that appears in the first three years of life, affecting boys more than girls in a ratio of approximately 4 : 1 [1]. One of the most consistent abnormalities in autism in the published literature since 1961 is elevated blood serotonin (see [2] for review). Autism may also be associated with altered metabolism of catecholamines (dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine), for example, elevated plasma levels of dopamine and epinephrine [3]. The monoamines (serotonin and catecholamines) are known to act not only as neurotransmitters, but also as signaling molecules in the gastrointestinal tract and immune system. Moreover, neurotransmitters may play a role in neurogenesis during brain development [4]. Thus, abnormal monoamine metabolism may have a profound impact on immune responses and gastrointestinal activities [57] as well as on neurodevelopment [8, 9]. From this point of view, it seems that disturbed monoamine metabolism, which is known to be caused by a variety of factors (e.g., drugs [9] and diet [10]), may play a crucial role in the development of autism. Thus, a better understanding of the mechanism of disturbed monoamine metabolism may provide insights into the etiology of autism.

Evidence suggests that the etiology of autism may involve both genetic and environmental factors [11, 12]. However, exactly what those environmental factors are remains to be determined. Notably, there were no significant pollution events in the United States from the 1980s and through the 1990s, but why was there a sudden increase in the incidence of autism among the 1987–1992 birth cohorts [13, 14]? If disturbed monoamine-neurotransmitter metabolism plays a role in the development of autism, factors accounting for the increased prevalence of autism could be those that can directly or indirectly affect monoamine-neurotransmitter metabolism. Some vitamins are known to increase the levels of monoamine neurotransmitters (see below). Excess vitamins are also known to have side effects like neurotoxicity [15]. Most significantly, over the past few decades, there has been a significant increase in multivitamin exposure in infancy due to high vitamin feeding and supplementation [16, 17]. Thus, the possibility exists that the increased incidence of autism may be related to excess multivitamin exposure. In this paper, we will focus on the relationship between excess multivitamin exposure and the inactivation/degradation of monoamine neurotransmitters and its possible role in the development of autism.

2. Monoamine-Neurotransmitter Inactivation

It is known that to ensure normal functioning of the nervous, immune, and digestive systems, the monoamines released from the nervous system and the gastrointestinal tract must be inactivated/degraded and eliminated in time. Monoamine-neurotransmitters, like xenobiotics (substances foreign to the body, such as pollutants, food additives, pesticides, and drugs), are metabolized through enzymatic phase I (oxidation, reduction, and hydrolysis) and phase II reactions (conjugation, e.g., methylation, sulfation, acetylation, glucuronidation, and glutathione conjugation) [18]. The characteristics of monoamine degradation are as follows: (1) enzymatic degradation: the degradation of all the monoamines and their precursor amino acids is enzymatic multipathway and multistep processes (Figure 1). The major enzymes involved in the degradation of monoamine neurotransmitters are monoamine oxidase (MAO), catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), acetylserotonin O-methyltransferase, and sulfotransferases, which are responsible for the oxidative deamination, methylation, and sulfation of the neurotransmitters, respectively. Genetic polymorphism of the enzymes has been demonstrated to contribute to interindividual differences in the overall metabolism of monoamines [19, 20]. In the degradation of monoamines and their precursors, when one pathway is interrupted, another pathway can partially compensate. For example, when the phenylalanine-tyrosine pathway is blocked by phenylalanine hydroxylase deficiency, phenylalanine is converted to phenylpyruvate, resulting in phenylketonuria [21].

963697.fig.001
Figure 1: The synthesis and degradation of serotonin and catecholamines. Oxidative deamination and methylation are two key mechanisms for inactivating monoamine neurotransmitters. Note that without labile methyl groups, methylation-mediated degradation of serotonin and catecholamines cannot take place, even though the methyltransferases involved are normal. 1, monoamine oxidase; 2, aldehyde dehydrogenase; 3, aldehyde reductase; 4, acetylserotonin O-methyltransferase; 5, catechol-O-methyltransferase. AADC: aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase; DHMA: 3,4-dihydroxymandelic acid; DHPG: 3,4-dihydroxyphenylglycol; DHR: dihydropteridine reductase; DOPAC: 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid; GTP: guanosine triphosphate; MHPG: 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol; PAH: phenylalanine hydroxylase; PNMT: phenylethanolamine N-methyltransferase; PPAT: phenylalanine(histidine):pyruvate aminotransferase; SNAT: serotonin N-acetyltransferase; TH, tyrosine-3-hydroxylase; TPH, tryptophan-5-hydroxylase; VMA: vanillylmandelic acid.

(2) Need for methyl groups and sulfate: as shown in Figure 2, methyl groups and sulfur amino acids (e.g., methionine and cysteine) are required for the body’s detoxification and antioxidant activities (Figure 2). An adequate supply of methyl groups and sulfate is prerequisite for methylation- and sulfation-mediated monoamine-neurotransmitter inactivation. Since both the biotransformation of exogenous chemicals and the degradation of monoamine neurotransmitters share the same pool of methyl groups [22] and sulfate [23], in theory, any chemicals (such as vitamins, see the following) that consume methyl groups and/or sulfur amino acids in their biotransformation may competitively inhibit the methylation and sulfation of monoamine neurotransmitters.

963697.fig.002
Figure 2: Role of methyl groups and sulfur amino acids in the detoxification system. Methylation, sulfation, glutathione conjugation, and glutathione-mediated antioxidant defense are major components of the detoxification system. An adequate supply of methyl donors, primarily betaine and choline, is a prerequisite for methylation reactions. Glutathione, formed from cysteine, is required for glutathione conjugation and the clearance of hydrogen peroxide and hydroxyl radicals, while 3′-phosphoadenosine 5′-phosphosulfate (PAPS), derived from the metabolism of sulfur amino acids, is the universal sulfate donor for all sulfation processes. Excess vitamins may disturb the degradation of monoamine neurotransmitters by depleting the body’s methyl-group and sulfate pools. 1, methyltransferases; 2, glutathione S-transferase; 3, phase I xenobiotic-metabolizing enzymes; 4, glutathione peroxidases; 5, glutathione reductase; 6, sulfotransferases. 5-CH3-THF: 5-methyltetrahydrofolate; DMG: dimethylglycine; GSH: reduced glutathione; GSSG: oxidized glutathione; PAP: 3′-phosphoadenosine-5′-phosphate; R-OH: alcohols; ROOH: organic peroxides (which generate free radicals); THF: tetrahydrofolate.

(3) Gender differences in monoamine-neurotransmitter inactivation: as mentioned above, monoamine neurotransmitters can be inactivated either by deamination, by methylation, or by sulfation. The redundant nature of monoamine-neurotransmitter metabolism enables one pathway to compensate for blockade of the other. For example, reduced or absent activity of MAO leads to a decrease in the production of deaminated metabolites and an increase in that of O-methylated amine metabolites [24, 25], while inhibition of COMT increases the production of 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid [25], a deaminated metabolite of dopamine (Figure 1). Thus, if methylation and sulfation cannot take place (e.g., due to depleted methyl-group and sulfate pools by exogenous chemicals) [22, 23], the inactivation of monoamine neurotransmitters will depend mainly on the activity of MAO. Importantly, the genes encoding the two isoforms of MAO are X-linked [26], and their activity is lower in males than in females [27, 28], suggesting a biological basis of sex differences in monoamine degradation. Such a sex difference in MAO activity also suggests that males might have less ability to compensate for blockade of methylation- and sulfation-mediated monoamine inactivation than females. Therefore, it is conceivable that similar levels of exogenous chemical exposure may disturb the inactivation of monoamine neurotransmitters in males more than in females.

3. Effect of Vitamins on Monoamine-Neurotransmitter Metabolism

Excess vitamins, like xenobiotics and monoamine neurotransmitters, are also degraded through phase I and phase II reactions and thus may increase the consumption of labile methyl-groups and sulfate. Moreover, some vitamins are known to play a role in the synthesis of monoamine neurotransmitters. For example, vitamin B6 is a cofactor for aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase that catalyzes the formation of serotonin and dopamine (Figure 1), while 5-methyltetrahydrofolate, the active form of folate, also stimulates the synthesis of monoamine neurotransmitters [29]. Therefore, excess vitamins can increase the levels of monoamine neurotransmitters either by competing for the same biotransformation system or by facilitating the synthesis, or by both. Indeed, evidence shows that high doses of vitamin C decrease plasma-conjugated dopamine and norepinephrine levels by competing for sulfation [30], whereas nicotinamide increases the levels of plasma of norepinephrine [31], serotonin, and histamine [32], presumably due to a decrease in methylation-mediated degradation of the monoamines. Vitamin B6 supplementation can increase the blood serotonin levels of newborn babies [33]. Interestingly, Berman and colleagues [34] found that maternal supplementation with vitamin B6 during the last 3 to 5 weeks of pregnancy increased the maternal blood levels of serotonin at parturition but did not increase the cord blood serotonin level or urinary 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid output in the newborn infants, suggesting that the placenta may protect the fetus from the risk of excess vitamin exposure. Although little is known about the effect of excess vitamin exposure on the neurotransmitter metabolism in the human infant brain, evidence from animal studies has shown that some vitamins can affect the metabolism of central monoamine neurotransmitters. For example, vitamin C [35] and vitamin B6 [36, 37] increase the levels of serotonin in the brain of rats. Recently, Tekes and colleagues [38] found that neonatal vitamin A or vitamin D treatment has significant influence on the metabolism of monoamine neurotransmitters in the adult rat brain. Therefore, excess vitamin exposure may be a potential risk factor for neurotransmitter metabolism disorders.

4. Toxicity of Excess Vitamins

It has been known for over a century that the dose-response curve for many micronutrients is nonmonotonic, having an initial stage of increasing benefits with increased intake, followed by increasing costs as excesses become toxic [39]. Both vitamin deficiency and vitamin excess are known to cause toxicity, including neurotoxicity [15, 40]. A meta-analysis of randomized trials of antioxidant supplements for primary and secondary prevention suggests that supplementation of vitamin A and E may increase mortality [41]. Supplemental folic acid (the synthetic form of folate) was also found to be associated with increased mortality [42, 43]. Davis and colleagues [44] found an association between high serum thiamine levels and sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS, a sudden and unexplained infant death most likely to occur between 2 and 4 months of age), and they further demonstrated that high doses of thiamine could cause death in rabbits and mice due to respiratory failure. Moreover, there is evidence suggesting an association between early infant vitamin supplementation and an increased risk of allergic diseases later in life [45, 46]. Although these data are not conclusive, they at least suggest the possibility that excess vitamin exposure may lead to serious health outcomes.

To date, little is known about the relationship between early infant exposure to excess vitamins and autism, except a recent hypothesis that suggests that excess folic acid supplementation may be a risk factor for autism [47]. There are two studies that examine the relationship between early vitamin exposure and learning development in rats. One found that neonatal vitamin A exposure may induce a long-lasting defect in learning [48], and the other showed that niacin supplementation induced spatial learning impairment in rats [49]. These observations suggest that early excess vitamin exposure may have adverse effects on neurodevelopment. It should be noted that the neurological effects of vitamin deficiency and vitamin excess may be similar [15]. Such a similarity could be a common cause for a wrong diagnosis. For example, SIDS was initially suggested to be related to a thiamine deficiency. To test this hypothesis, Davis and colleagues [44] compared serum thiamine levels between 233 SIDS infants and 46 control infants dying from other causes. Unexpectedly, they found that most of the SIDS infants had significantly higher serum thiamine levels. Therefore, to avoid making a wrong diagnosis, the levels of vitamins and their metabolites should be monitored.

It should be pointed out that some cofactors, although not belonging to vitamins, may also play an important role in the synthesis of monoamine neurotransmitters. As shown in Figure 1, tetrahydrobiopterin, which is synthesized from guanosine triphosphate, is an essential cofactor for dopamine and serotonin biosynthesis. Thus, it is conceivable that excess tetrahydrobiopterin can increase monoamine-neurotransmitter levels and may contribute to monoamine-related mental disorders. Indeed, evidence has shown that tetrahydrobiopterin may cause preferential death of catecholaminergic cells, presumably due to increased dopamine levels [50]. However, it is unlikely that tetrahydrobiopterin may play a role in the rapidly increased prevalence of autism in the past few decades, since there is no evidence suggesting an increase in the synthesis of tetrahydrobiopterin in the early infancy of autistic patients, or in the content of tetrahydrobiopterin in infant foods.

5. High Multivitamin Exposure and Increased Autism Prevalence

For decades, since it was first described by Kanner in 1943 [52], the prevalence of autism in the United States was low. Autism prevalence studies published before 1985 showed prevalence rates of 4 to 5 per 10,000 children for the broader autism spectrum and about 2 per 10,000 for the classic autism definition [53]. Since 1985, there have been higher rates of autism, with the greatest annual increases occurring between the 1987 and 1992 birth cohorts [14]. During this period, there were no significant national environmental events but a significant event related to infant feeding. In 1988, US formula companies removed the realm of infant feeding from the exclusive supervision of the medical profession and targeted an advertising campaign for their formula products at the general public [51]. If formulas played a role in increased autism prevalence, the campaign should have been followed by several consecutive years of rapid increase in autism cases. In fact, the sharp increase in autism prevalence in annual cohorts born from 1987 to 1992 in California (Figure 3) and the whole of the United States [14] occurred simultaneously with the initiation of the direct advertising of infant formula. The prevalence of autism among 6-year-old children increased from 4.6 per 10,000 in the 1986 birth cohort to 19.1 per 10,000 in the 1992 birth cohort in the United States [14]. Moreover, there are studies showing that premature weaning and suboptimal breast-feeding practices are associated with increased risk of autism [54, 55]. Thus, it appears that the risk factor(s) for autism may be present in infant foods.

963697.fig.003
Figure 3: Events related to infant feeding and the prevalence of autism. The data on the number of cases of children with autism in the 1980–1994 birth cohorts in California are taken from [13]. Note that the implementation the Infant Formula Act of 1980 [51] was followed by an apparent increase in autism incidence, while the advertising of infant formula to the general public, started in 1988, was accompanied with a rapid increase in the incidence of autism. The increases were greatest for annual cohorts born from 1987 to 1992.

Among the possible risk factors in infant foods, such as nutritional imbalances (deficiencies and excesses) and food additives and contaminates, excess multivitamin exposure may be the most common and important. In order to insure the safety of formulas, the United States implemented the Infant Formula Act of 1980, which sets a lower limit of vitamins in infant formulas but does not set an upper limit for most vitamins [56]. This has caused concern that, without upper limits, super-fortified formula could be produced [57] and may have a direct toxic effect [58]. In fact, infant formulas, especially those for premature infants, generally contain much higher levels of vitamins. For example, the content of niacin, folic acid, vitamin B6, thiamine, and vitamin C in a premature infant formula (see, http://abbottnutrition.com/products/similac-special-care-20-with-iron) is about 20 (5,000 versus 250 μg/100 kcal), 9 (37 versus 4 μg/100 kcal), 7 (250 versus 35 μg/100 kcal), 6 (250 versus 40 μg/100 kcal), and 4 (37 versus 8 mg/100 kcal) times the lower limit value, respectively. The level of thiamine in some manufactured milk-based formulae (2160 μg/L) was found to be about 20 times that of human breast milk (mean 178 μg/L) [44]. In addition to the vitamin supplementation of infant formula, multivitamin use in infants and toddlers is very common [16]. Thus, high-vitamin feeding may increase the risk of vitamin overload. Indeed, many studies have shown that formula-fed infants have higher levels plasma/serum of vitamins than human milk-fed infants [5962]. Unmetabolized folic acid, a sign of folic acid overload, is observed in the serum of 4-day-old infants fed with formula [63]. Porcelli and colleagues [62] found a several times increase in the plasma levels of riboflavin and pyridoxine and a more than 10 times increase in the urine riboflavin and pyridoxine concentrations in very low-birth-weight neonates after being fed with preterm infant formula. Baeckert and colleagues [64] showed that very low-birth-weight infants who received the recommended parenteral vitamin supplement as part of their total parenteral nutrition developed elevated plasma riboflavin concentrations during their first postnatal month with peak concentrations 100-fold above baseline umbilical cord plasma vitamin concentrations. Moreover, there are two studies finding high plasma levels and urinary excretion of methylated metabolites of niacin in autistic patients [65, 66], which suggests a niacin overload, because excess niacin is rapidly degraded after ingestion, but its methylated metabolites remain longer in the circulation [31, 67]. Given that excess vitamins may lead to neurotoxicity and disturbances in monoamine neurotransmitter metabolism, as mentioned earlier, it is possible that high multivitamin exposure may play a role in the increased prevalence of autism.

6. Critical Window of Vulnerability for Autism

Brain overgrowth has been noted among children with autism [68]. An understanding of the timing of brain enlargement in autism may be particularly helpful in identifying the window of vulnerability for autism. Hazlett and colleagues [69] observed generalized cerebral cortical enlargement in individuals with autism at both 2 and 4 to 5 years of age, but they found that there was no significant difference from controls in the rate of brain growth for this age interval, indicating that brain enlargement in autism results from an increased rate of brain growth before age of 2 years. A recent study using diffusion tensor imaging showed that there have been significant brain differences at age of 6 months between high-risk infants who later develop autism and those who did not [70], clearly indicating that autism develops over time during infancy. Moreover, studies found that premature weaning and suboptimal breast-feeding practices may increase the risk of autism [54, 55]. The above two lines of evidence suggest that the first few months after birth could be a critical window of vulnerability for autism.

Current understanding of the rates of maturation of metabolic capability indicates that human infants up to approximately 6 months of age are typically more sensitive to chemical toxicity than adults due to their immature detoxification systems [71]. This suggests that newborn infants, especially premature infants, may have a low tolerance to excess vitamins. Indeed, available evidence, although limited, has shown an association between high levels of some vitamins (thiamine [44] and vitamin C [72]) and apparent life-threatening events and SIDS in infancy. A randomized controlled trial on vitamin C supplementation in very preterm infants also showed that infants who died during the trial were those who had significantly higher vitamin C concentrations before randomization than surviving infants [73]. Evidence from animal studies suggests that high exposure to vitamin A [48] and niacin [49] in the early life has adverse effects on the behaviors of adult rats. Thus, it appears that high multivitamin feeding in the first few months of life may be particularly harmful. Although there is little information on the role of excess vitamins in infant brain injuries, it is common knowledge that chemical exposure-induced neurological injury may have a variety of manifestations, depending on the length and degree of exposure [74]. Notably, preterm birth is associated with increased risk for both autism and other neurological conditions, such as cognitive, visual, and hearing impairments; and there is considerable cooccurrence of autism with other neurological and cognitive disorders [75]. We therefore postulate that autism might be one of chemical/excess-vitamin exposure-induced neurological sequelae (which may range from neurological deficits to death) in early infancy. What is worthy of note is that with the maturation of metabolic function and age-related changes in feeding foods, the causal exposure present in infancy and resultant metabolic and neurological manifestations may no longer exist. This may account for why there is lack of consistent biological markers in autism. Even elevated blood serotonin, the most consistent serotonin-related finding in autism, may not be observed in adolescent autistic patients [76].

7. Conclusion

Given that (1) high multivitamin feeding is very common in early infancy, (2) excess vitamins may cause neurotoxicity and disturb monoamine-neurotransmitter metabolism, and (3) autism is often associated with abnormal levels of monoamine neurotransmitters, it seems that excess multivitamin exposure in early infancy may be a potential risk factor for autism. Further studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Acknowledgment

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundations of China (nos. 31140036 and 81000575).

References

  1. Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators, “Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 sites, United States, 2008,” MMWR Surveillance Summaries, vol. 61, no. 3, pp. 1–19, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  2. C. J. McDougle, C. A. Erickson, K. A. Stigler, and D. J. Posey, “Neurochemistry in the pathophysiology of autism,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 66, supplement 10, pp. 9–18, 2005. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  3. J. Martineau, J. Hérault, E. Petit et al., “Catecholaminergic metabolism and autism,” Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, vol. 36, no. 8, pp. 688–697, 1994. View at Google Scholar
  4. J. C. Platel, S. Stamboulian, I. Nguyen, and A. Bordey, “Neurotransmitter signaling in postnatal neurogenesis: the first leg,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 63, no. 1-2, pp. 60–71, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  5. W. I. Khan and J. E. Ghia, “Gut hormones: emerging role in immune activation and inflammation,” Clinical and Experimental Immunology, vol. 161, no. 1, pp. 19–27, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  6. R. Spiller, “Serotonin and GI clinical disorders,” Neuropharmacology, vol. 55, no. 6, pp. 1072–1080, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  7. R. Oberbeck, “Catecholamines: physiological immunomodulators during health and illness,” Current Medicinal Chemistry, vol. 13, no. 17, pp. 1979–1989, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  8. D. C. Chugani, “Serotonin in autism and pediatric epilepsies,” Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 112–116, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  9. A. L. Frederick and G. D. Stanwood, “Drugs, biogenic amine targets and the developing brain,” Developmental Neuroscience, vol. 31, no. 1-2, pp. 7–22, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  10. I. Blum, Y. Vered, E. Graff et al., “The influence of meal composition on plasma serotonin and norepinephrine concentrations,” Metabolism: Clinical and Experimental, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 137–140, 1992. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  11. J. Hallmayer, S. Cleveland, A. Torres et al., “Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 68, no. 11, pp. 1095–1102, 2011. View at Google Scholar
  12. P. J. Landrigan, L. Lambertini, and L. S. Birnbaum, “A research strategy to discover the environmental causes of autism and neurodevelopmental disabilities,” Environmental Health Perspectives, vol. 120, no. 7, pp. a258–a260, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  13. L. Dales, S. J. Hammer, and N. J. Smith, “Time trends in autism and in MMR immunization coverage in California,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 285, no. 9, pp. 1183–1185, 2001. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  14. C. J. Newschaffer, M. D. Falb, and J. G. Gurney, “National autism prevalence trends from United States special education data,” Pediatrics, vol. 115, no. 3, pp. e277–e282, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  15. S. R. Snodgrass, “Vitamin neurotoxicity,” Molecular Neurobiology, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 41–73, 1992. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  16. D. M. Curtis, “Infant nutrient supplementation,” Journal of Pediatrics, vol. 117, part 2, pp. S110–S118, 1990. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  17. R. Briefel, C. Hanson, M. K. Fox, T. Novak, and P. Ziegler, “Feeding infants and toddlers study: do vitamin and mineral supplements contribute to nutrient adequacy or excess among US infants and toddlers?” Journal of the American Dietetic Association, vol. 106, supplement 1, pp. S52–S65, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  18. F. J. Gonzalez and R. H. Turkey, “Drug metabolism,” in Goodman and Gilman’s Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics, pp. 71–92, Medical Publishing Division, New York, NY, USA, 11th edition, 2006. View at Google Scholar
  19. J. Volavka, R. Bilder, and K. Nolan, “Catecholamines and aggression: the role of COMT and MAO polymorphisms,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1036, pp. 393–398, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  20. R. B. Raftogianis, T. C. Wood, and R. M. Weinshilboum, “Human phenol sulfotransferases SULT1A2 and SULT1A1 Genetic polymorphisms, allozyme properties, and human liver genotype-phenotype correlations,” Biochemical Pharmacology, vol. 58, no. 4, pp. 605–616, 1999. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  21. N. Blau, F. J. Van Spronsen, and H. L. Levy, “Phenylketonuria,” The Lancet, vol. 376, no. 9750, pp. 1417–1427, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  22. S. S. Zhou, Y. M. Zhou, D. Li, and Y. Z. Lun, “Dietary methyl-consuming compounds and metabolic syndrome,” Hypertension Research, vol. 34, no. 12, pp. 1239–1245, 2011. View at Google Scholar
  23. M. W. H. Coughtrie, K. J. Bamforth, S. Sharp et al., “Sulfation of endogenous compounds and xenobiotics—interactions and function in health and disease,” Chemico-Biological Interactions, vol. 92, no. 1-3, pp. 247–256, 1994. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  24. J. W. M. Lenders, G. Eisenhofer, N. G. G. M. Abeling et al., “Specific genetic deficiencies of the A and B isoenzymes of monoamine oxidase are characterized by distinct neurochemical and clinical phenotypes,” Journal of Clinical Investigation, vol. 97, no. 4, pp. 1010–1019, 1996. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  25. M. Okada, R. Nakao, R. Hosoi et al., “Microdialysis with radiometric monitoring of L-[β-11C] DOPA to assess dopaminergic metabolism: effect of inhibitors of L-amino acid decarboxylase, monoamine oxidase, and catechol-O-methyltransferase on rat striatal dialysate,” Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 124–131, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  26. N. C. Lan, C. Heinzmann, A. Gal et al., “Human monoamine oxidase A and B genes map to xp11.23 and are deleted in a patient with norrie disease,” Genomics, vol. 4, no. 4, pp. 552–559, 1989. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  27. D. S. Robinson, J. M. Davis, A. Nies, C. L. Ravaris, and D. Sylwester, “Relation of sex and aging to monoamine oxidase activity of human brain, plasma, and platelets,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 24, no. 6, pp. 536–539, 1971. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  28. M. Harro, D. Eensoo, E. Kiive et al., “Platelet monoamine oxidase in healthy 9- and 15-years old children: the effect of gender, smoking and puberty,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, vol. 25, no. 8, pp. 1497–1511, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  29. S. M. Stahl, “L-methylfolate: A vitamin for your monoamines,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 69, no. 9, pp. 1352–1353, 2008. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  30. J. W. Dunne, L. Davidson, and R. Vandongen, “The effect of ascorbic acid on the sulphate conjugation of ingested noradrenaline and dopamine,” British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 356–360, 1984. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  31. W. P. Sun, D. Li, Y. Z. Lun et al., “Excess nicotinamide inhibits methylation-mediated degradation of catecholamines in normotensives and hypertensives,” Hypertension Research, vol. 35, no. 2, pp. 180–185, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  32. Y. J. Tian, D. Li, Q. Ma et al., “Excess nicotinamide increases plasma serotonin and histamine levels,” Acta Physiologica Sinica, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 33–38, 2013. View at Google Scholar
  33. J. L. Berman, P. Justice, and D. Y. Y. Hsia, “The metabolism of 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) in the newborn,” The Journal of Pediatrics, vol. 67, no. 4, pp. 603–608, 1965. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  34. J. L. Berman, “Effect of maternal pyridoxine ingestion on cord blood 5-hydroxytryptamine concentration.,” Archives of Disease in Childhood, vol. 41, no. 215, pp. 51–53, 1966. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  35. L. Lee, S. A. Kang, H. O. Lee et al., “Effect of supplementation of vitamin E and vitamin C on brain acetylcholinesterase activity and neurotransmitter levels in rats treated with scopolamine, an inducer of dementia,” Journal of Nutritional Science and Vitaminology, vol. 47, no. 5, pp. 323–328, 2001. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  36. K. Dakshinamurti, S. K. Sharma, and D. Bonke, “Influence of B vitamins on binding properties of serotonin receptors in the CNS of rats,” Klinische Wochenschrift, vol. 68, no. 2, pp. 142–145, 1990. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  37. D. Calderón-Guzmán, J. L. Hernández-Islas, I. Espitia-Vázquez et al., “Pyridoxine, regardless of serotonin levels, increases production of 5-hydroxytryptophan in rat brain,” Archives of Medical Research, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 271–274, 2004. View at Google Scholar
  38. K. Tekes, M. Gyenge, A. Folyovich, and G. Csaba, “Influence of neonatal vitamin A or vitamin D treatment on the concentration of biogenic amines and their metabolites in the adult rat brain,” Hormone and Metabolic Research, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 277–280, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  39. D. Raubenheimer, K. P. Lee, and S. J. Simpson, “Does Bertrand's rule apply to macronutrients?” Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 272, no. 1579, pp. 2429–2434, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  40. D. J. Lanska, “Chapter 30 Historical aspects of the major neurological vitamin deficiency disorders: the water-soluble B vitamins,” Handbook of Clinical Neurology, vol. 95, pp. 445–476, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  41. G. Bjelakovic, D. Nikolova, L. L. Gluud, R. G. Simonetti, and C. Gluud, “Mortality in randomized trials of antioxidant supplements for primary and secondary prevention: systematic review and meta-analysis,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 297, no. 8, pp. 842–857, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  42. M. Ebbing, K. H. Bønaa, O. Nygård et al., “Cancer incidence and mortality after treatment with folic acid and vitamin B12,” The Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 302, no. 19, pp. 2119–2126, 2009. View at Google Scholar
  43. N. Roswall, A. Olsen, J. Christensen et al., “Micronutrient intake in relation to all-cause mortality in a prospective Danish cohort,” Food & Nutrition Research, vol. 56, article 5466, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  44. R. E. Davis, G. C. Icke, and J. M. Hilton, “High serum thiamine and the sudden infant death syndrome,” Clinica Chimica Acta, vol. 123, no. 3, pp. 321–328, 1982. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  45. J. D. Milner, D. M. Stein, R. McCarter, and R. Y. Moon, “Early infant multivitamin supplementation is associated with increased risk for food allergy and asthma,” Pediatrics, vol. 114, no. 1, pp. 27–32, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  46. E. Hyppönen, U. Sovio, M. Wjst et al., “Infant vitamin D supplementation and allergic conditions in adulthood: Northern Finland birth cohort 1966,” Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 1037, pp. 84–95, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  47. C. M. Beard, L. A. Panser, and S. K. Katusic, “Is excess folic acid supplementation a risk factor for autism?” Medical Hypotheses, vol. 77, no. 1, pp. 15–17, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  48. T. Kihara, T. Matsuo, M. Sakamoto, Y. Yasuda, and T. Tanimura, “Effects of the neonatal vitamin A exposure on behaviors of adult rats,” Journal of Toxicological Sciences, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 93–101, 1995. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  49. G. S. Young, E. L. Jacobson, and J. B. Kirkland, “Water maze performance in young male Long-Evans rats is inversely affected by dietary intakes of niacin and may be linked to levels of the NAD+ metabolite cADPR,” Journal of Nutrition, vol. 137, no. 4, pp. 1050–1057, 2007. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  50. H. J. Choi, S. W. Kim, S. Y. Lee, and O. Hwang, “Dopamine-dependent cytotoxicity of tetrahydrobiopterin: a possible mechanism for selective neurodegeneration in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Neurochemistry, vol. 86, no. 1, pp. 143–152, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  51. F. R. Greer and R. D. Apple, “Physicians, formula companies, and advertising: a historical perspective,” American Journal of Diseases of Children, vol. 145, no. 3, pp. 282–286, 1991. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  52. L. Kanner, “Autistic disturbances of affective contact,” Nervous Child, vol. 2, pp. 217–250, 1943. View at Google Scholar
  53. J. Merrick, I. Kandel, and M. Morad, “Trends in austism,” International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 75–78, 2004. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  54. Y. Tanoue and S. Oda, “Weaning time of children with infantile autism,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 425–434, 1989. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  55. Y. M. Al-Farsi, M. M. Al-Sharbati, M. I. Waly et al., “Effect of suboptimal breast-feeding on occurrence of autism: a case-control study,” Nutrition, vol. 28, no. 7-8, pp. e27–e32, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  56. United States Congress, “Infant formula act of 1980,” in Public Law, pp. 96–359, United States Capitol Health Documents Room, Washington, DC, USA, 1980. View at Google Scholar
  57. D. A. Cook, “Nutrient levels in infant formulas: technical considerations,” Journal of Nutrition, vol. 119, supplement 12, pp. 1773–1778, 1989. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  58. B. A. Wharton, “An approach to setting maxima in infant formulas,” Journal of Nutrition, vol. 119, supplement 12, pp. 1768–1772, 1989. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  59. A. M. Smith, M. F. Picciano, and R. H. Deering, “Folate intake and blood concentrations of term infants,” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 590–598, 1985. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  60. R. E. Davis, G. C. Icke, J. M. Hilton, and E. Orr, “Serum thiamin, pyridoxal, cobalamin and folate concentrations in young infants,” Acta Paediatrica Scandinavica, vol. 75, no. 3, pp. 402–407, 1986. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  61. K. Heiskanen, L. Salmenperä, J. Perheentupa, and M. A. Siimes, “Infant vitamin B-6 status changes with age and with formula feeding,” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, vol. 60, no. 6, pp. 907–910, 1994. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  62. P. J. Porcelli, E. W. Adcock, D. DelPaggio, L. L. Swift, and H. L. Greene, “Plasma and urine riboflavin and pyridoxine concentrations in enterally fed very-low-birth-weight neonates,” Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 141–146, 1996. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  63. M. R. Sweeney, J. McPartlin, D. G. Weir et al., “Evidence of unmetabolised folic acid and cord blood of newborn and serum of 4-day-old infants,” British Journal of Nutrition, vol. 94, no. 5, pp. 727–730, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  64. P. A. Baeckert, H. L. Greene, I. Fritz, D. G. Oelberg, and E. W. Adcock, “Vitamin concentrations in very low birth weight infants given viatamins intravenously in a lipid emulsion: measurement of vitamins A, D, and E and riboflavin,” Journal of Pediatrics, vol. 113, no. 6, pp. 1057–1065, 1988. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  65. I. K. S. Yap, M. Angley, K. A. Veselkov, E. Holmes, J. C. Lindon, and J. K. Nicholson, “Urinary metabolic phenotyping differentiates children with autism from their unaffected siblings and age-matched controls,” Journal of Proteome Research, vol. 9, no. 6, pp. 2996–3004, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  66. J. B. Adams, T. Audhya, S. McDonough-Means et al., “Nutritional and metabolic status of children with autism vs. neurotypical children, and the association with autism severity,” Nutrition & Metabolism, vol. 8, article 34, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  67. S. S. Zhou, D. Li, W. P. Sun et al., “Nicotinamide overload may play a role in the development of type 2 diabetes,” World Journal of Gastroenterology, vol. 15, no. 45, pp. 5674–5684, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  68. H. C. Hazlett, M. Poe, G. Gerig et al., “Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism: birth through age 2 years,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 62, no. 12, pp. 1366–1376, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  69. H. C. Hazlett, M. D. Poe, G. Gerig et al., “Early brain overgrowth in autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before age 2 years,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 68, no. 5, pp. 467–476, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  70. J. J. Wolff, H. Gu, G. Gerig et al., “Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 169, no. 6, pp. 589–600, 2012. View at Google Scholar
  71. R. Scheuplein, G. Charnley, and M. Dourson, “Differential sensitivity of children and adults to chemical toxicity. I. Biological basis,” Regulatory Toxicology and Pharmacology, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 429–447, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  72. A. Dick and R. Ford, “Cholinergic and oxidative stress mechanisms in sudden infant death syndrome,” Acta Paediatrica, International Journal of Paediatrics, vol. 98, no. 11, pp. 1768–1775, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  73. B. A. Darlow, H. Buss, F. McGill, L. Fletcher, P. Graham, and C. C. Winterbourn, “Vitamin C supplementation in very preterm infants: a randomised controlled trial,” Archives of Disease in Childhood, vol. 90, no. 2, pp. F117–F122, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  74. A. Miodovnik, “Environmental neurotoxicants and developing brain,” Mount Sinai Journal of Medicine, vol. 78, no. 1, pp. 58–77, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  75. K. C. K. Kuban, T. M. O'Shea, E. N. Allred, H. Tager-Flusberg, D. J. Goldstein, and A. Leviton, “Positive screening on the modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT) in extremely low gestational age newborns,” Journal of Pediatrics, vol. 154, no. 4, pp. 535–540, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  76. J. Croonenberghs, L. Delmeire, R. Verkerk et al., “Peripheral markers of serotonergic and noradrenergic function in post- pubertal, caucasian males with autistic disorder,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 275–283, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus