Table of Contents Author Guidelines Submit a Manuscript
Child Development Research
Volume 2015, Article ID 189195, 15 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/189195
Research Article

Early Vocabulary Development in Rural and Urban Mozambique

1Tilburg Centre for Cognition and Communication, Tilburg University, P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, Netherlands
2Stanford University Developmental Psychology, Language Learning Lab, 50 Serra Mall, Margaret Jacks Building, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
3Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, University Road, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK

Received 31 July 2015; Revised 12 October 2015; Accepted 10 November 2015

Academic Editor: Magdalena Janus

Copyright © 2015 Paul Vogt et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Linked References

  1. L. Fenson, P. S. Dale, J. S. Reznick et al., The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual, Singular Publishing Group, San Diego, Calif, USA, 1993.
  2. M. H. Bornstein, L. R. Cote, S. Maital et al., “Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English,” Child Development, vol. 75, no. 4, pp. 1115–1139, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  3. M. C. Caselli, E. Bates, P. Casadio et al., “A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development,” Cognitive Development, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 159–199, 1995. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  4. A. Fernald, V. A. Marchman, and A. Weisleder, “SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months,” Developmental Science, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 234–248, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  5. D. Jackson-Maldonado, V. Marchman, and L. Fernald, “Short-form versions of the Spanish MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories,” Applied Psycholinguistics, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 837–868, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  6. P. S. Dale and M. Penford, “Adaptations of the MacArthur-bates CDI into non-U.S. English languages,” Unpublished Survey, 2011. View at Google Scholar
  7. K. Alcock and N. Alibhai, “Language development in Sub-Saharan Africa,” in Neuropsychology of Children in Africa, pp. 155–180, Springer, Berlin, Germany, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  8. J. B. Childers, J. Vaughan, and D. A. Burquest, “Joint attention and word learning in Ngas-speaking toddlers in Nigeria,” Journal of Child Language, vol. 34, no. 2, pp. 199–225, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  9. L. Fenson, S. Pethick, C. Renda, J. L. Cox, P. S. Dale, and J. S. Reznick, “Short-form versions of the MacArthur communicative development inventories,” Applied Psycholinguistics, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 95–116, 2000. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  10. P. Vogt and J. D. Mastin, “Rural and urban differences in language socialization and early vocabulary development in Mozambique,” in Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci '13), M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, and I. Wachsmuth, Eds., pp. 3787–3792, Cognitive Science Society, Austin, Tex, USA, July-August 2013.
  11. J. D. Mastin and P. Vogt, “Infant engagement and early vocabulary development: a naturalistic observation study of Mozambican infants from 1;1 to 2;1,” Journal of Child Language, 30 pages, 2015. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  12. P. Vogt, J. D. Mastin, and D. M. A. Schots, “Communicative intentions of child-directed speech in three different learning environments: observations from the Netherlands, and rural and urban Mozambique,” First Language, vol. 35, no. 4-5, pp. 341–358, 2015. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  13. M. Da Conceição, “A brief look at the sociolinguistics of Ronga and other languages spoken in Mozambique,” University of Washington Working Papers in Linguistics 16, University of Washington, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  14. M. P. Lewis, Ethnologue: Languages of the World, SIL International, 2009.
  15. T. Tardif, P. Fletcher, W. Liang, and N. Kaciroti, “Early vocabulary development in Mandarin (Putonghua) and Cantonese,” Journal of Child Language, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 1115–1144, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  16. G. Szagun, B. Stumper, and S. A. Schramm, FRAKIS: Fragebogen zur Frühkindlichen Sprachentwicklung (FRAKIS) und FRAKIS-K (Kurzform), Pearson, 2009.
  17. M. Eriksson, M. Westerlund, and E. Berglund, “A screening version of the Swedish communicative development inventories designed for use with 18-month-old children,” Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 948–960, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  18. E. L. Prado, K. J. Alcock, M. T. Ullman, J. Phuka, B. Yakes, and M. Arimond, “Assessing child development in the context of nutriation trials in developing countries: principles of test selection, adaptation and evaluation,” in Proceedings of the Lipid-Based Nutrient Supplements Research Network Meeting, Washington, DC, USA, 2011.
  19. K. J. Alcock, K. Rimba, P. Holding, P. Kitsao-Wekulo, A. Abubakar, and C. R. J. C. Newton, “Developmental inventories using illiterate parents as informants: Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) adaptation for two Kenyan languages,” Journal of Child Language, vol. 42, no. 4, pp. 763–785, 2015. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  20. F. van de Vijver, “Meta-analysis of cross-cultural comparisons of cognitive test performance,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, vol. 28, no. 6, pp. 678–709, 1997. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  21. J. D. Hamadani, H. Baker-Henningham, F. Tofail, F. Mehrin, S. N. Huda, and S. M. Grantham-McGregor, “Validity and reliability of mothers' reports of language development in 1-year-old children in a large-scale survey in Bangladesh,” Food & Nutrition Bulletin, vol. 31, supplement 2, pp. S198–S206, 2010. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  22. E. Bates and B. MacWhinney, “Mechanisms of language acquisition,” in Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, B. MacWhinney, Ed., pp. 157–193, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1987. View at Google Scholar
  23. D. A. Junker and I. J. Stockman, “Expressive vocabulary of German-English bilingual toddlers,” American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 381–394, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  24. B. Z. Pearson, S. C. Fernández, and D. K. Oller, “Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms,” Language Learning, vol. 43, no. 1, pp. 93–120, 1993. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  25. P. M. Greenfield, “Linking social change and developmental change: shifting pathways of human development,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 401–418, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  26. H. Keller, “Autonomy and relatedness revisited: cultural manifestations of universal human needs,” Child Development Perspectives, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 12–18, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  27. B. Hart and T. R. Risley, Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children, Paul H Brookes Publishing, 1995.
  28. E. Hoff, “The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech,” Child Development, vol. 74, no. 5, pp. 1368–1378, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  29. R. LeVine, S. Dixon, S. LeVine et al., Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1996.
  30. L. Fenson, E. Bates, P. Dale, V. Marchman, J. Reznick, and D. Thal, MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company, Baltimore, Md, USA, 2nd edition, 2007.
  31. E. Bates, D. Thal, D. Trauner et al., “From first words to grammar in children with focal brain injury,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 13, no. 3, pp. 275–343, 1997. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  32. S. P. Walker, T. D. Wachs, J. M. Gardner et al., “Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries,” The Lancet, vol. 369, no. 9556, pp. 145–157, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  33. S. Harkness, “spects of social environment and first language acquisition in rural Africa,” in Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition, pp. 309–316, 1977. View at Google Scholar
  34. B. Sitoe, N. Mahumana, and P. Langa, Dicionário Ronga-Português, Centre of Research and Promotion of Traditional Medicine (CIPROMETRA), Maputo, Mozambique, 2008.
  35. M. L. Rowe, Ş. Özçalişkan, and S. Goldin-Meadow, “Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development,” First language, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 182–199, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  36. M. L. Rowe and S. Goldin-Meadow, “Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry,” Science, vol. 323, no. 5916, pp. 951–953, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  37. M. Tomasello and J. Todd, “Joint attention and lexical acquisition style,” First Language, vol. 4, no. 12, pp. 197–211, 1983. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  38. E. Lieven, “Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children,” in Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition, C. Gallaway and B. J. Richards, Eds., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1994. View at Google Scholar
  39. L. A. Shneidman, M. E. Arroyo, S. C. Levine, and S. Goldin-Meadow, “What counts as effective input for word learning?” Journal of Child Language, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 672–686, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  40. L. Kirk, “Cross-cultural measurement of maternal behavior in mother-child teaching interaction,” Quality & Quantity, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 127–143, 1976. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  41. M. Friend and M. Keplinger, “Reliability and validity of the Computerized Comprehension Task (CCT): data from American English and Mexican Spanish infants,” Journal of Child Language, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 77–98, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  42. L. Dunn, L. Dunn, S. Bulheller, and H. Häcker, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, Minn, USA, 1965.
  43. D. H. S. Moçambique, “Inquérito demográfico e de saúde,” Tech. Rep., Instituto Nacional deEstatística, Ministério de Saúde, 2013. View at Google Scholar
  44. S. Grantham-McGregor, Y. B. Cheung, S. Cueto, P. Glewwe, L. Richter, and B. Strupp, “Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries,” The Lancet, vol. 369, no. 9555, pp. 60–70, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus