Table of Contents Author Guidelines Submit a Manuscript
International Journal of Alzheimer's Disease
Volume 2009 (2009), Article ID 638145, 6 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.4061/2009/638145
Clinical Study

Applying New Research Criteria for Diagnosis of Early Alzheimer's Disease: Sex and Intelligence Matter

1Department of Psychiatry, Mental Health and Old Age Psychiatry, Charité Medical University, 14050 Berlin, Germany
2Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, 89081 Ulm, Germany
3Division of Mental Health & Old Age Psychiatry, University of Ulm, Ludwig-Heilmeyer Strasse 2, Ulm, 89312 Günzburg, Germany

Received 30 March 2009; Accepted 9 July 2009

Academic Editor: Ricardo Nitrini

Copyright © 2009 U. Beinhoff et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Alzheimer's disease (AD) can be diagnosed according to new research criteria proposed recently (Dubois et al., 2007). Diagnosis is made on grounds of episodic memory deficits and one pathological biomarker: cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) or structural/functional imaging. Goal was to investigate the dependence of episodic memory function on material (verbal, visuospatial), gender and premorbid intellectual ability (IQ). The new research criteria of AD were applied retrospectively using data of 68 patients (Mini-Mental-Status Examination, MMSE 22) from a university memory clinic. Women with lower IQ performed worse on visuospatial episodic memory than women with higher IQ and men with the same IQ. Thus, women with lower IQ appear to be particularly vulnerable to visuospatial episodic memory deficits despite similar CSF tau values indicating a similar activity of the neurodegenerative process. Gender, premorbid IQ, and visuospatial material need to be considered in the assessment of episodic memory breakdown applying the newly proposed research criteria for the diagnosis of AD.

1. Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is one of the most common diseases of old age [13]. Established research criteria for clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) require diagnosis of dementia syndrome and exclusion of possible causes of dementia other than AD [4]. Recently, revised research criteria for positive diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease—with or without clinical dementia syndrome—were proposed [5]. Other than the previous set of criteria diagnosis is not performed by exclusion; the new set of criteria aim for a positive diagnosis of AD. The revised criteria demand a significant episodic memory impairment which does not improve with cuing plus an abnormal result for at least one biomarker: mediotemporal atrophy on structural neuroimaging, an AD-typical pattern on functional neuroimaging (PET or SPECT), or AD-typical results for Abeta- and tau-protein in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) analysis. With this framework, diagnosis of AD does not require presence of dementia syndrome.

The correlation between severity of dementia and neuropathology is loose [68]. One likely cause is that the disease is driven by diffusible oligomeric amyloid assemblies causing synaptic failure rather than by the amyloid depositions alone [9, 10]. The clinical presentation of failure of synaptic transmission in affected individuals, however, varies with premorbid variables [11]. Epidemiologic evidence suggests that individuals with higher IQ [12], or higher educational or occupational attainment [13, 14], or participation in leisure activities [15] have an increased capacity for buffering the burden of disease. Not only the capacity against decline may differ according to these variables but also the pattern of decline [16] which in addition may be gender-dependent [1719].

Investigations on healthy controls suggest a female advantage in episodic memory with predominantly verbal material and a male advantage for visuospatial tasks [2030]. The impact of gender on memory in aging, however, further is modified by general intellectual abilities [31].

It was the goal of the present study to analyze the influence of both, gender and general intellectual abilities on verbal and visuospatial episodic memory performance in patients diagnosed with newly revised research criteria that require at least one positive biomarker evidence for securing AD diagnosis [5].

2. Methods

The study was done in accordance with the ethical standards of the University Clinics (Berlin and Ulm). All persons gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

2.1. Study Sample

From the cohort of patients assessed in a specialized dementia outpatient university hospital service 68 patients with mild cognitive deficits (Mini-Mental-Status Examination, MMSE   22) were selected fulfilling either the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s Disease according to NINCDS-ADRDA criteria and DSM-IV or the criteria for Mild Cognitive Impairment as defined by Petersen [32]. Patients were only selected that contained necessary data (IQ, neuropsychology, CSF (tau-protein, Abeta-protein), or cerebral imaging) to apply the revised research criteria for probable Alzheimer's disease [5]. For final analysis patients were included when they had an episodic memory deficit that does not significantly improve with cuing plus at least one supportive finding (either abnormal CSF tau or Abeta concentrations or medial temporal lobe (MTL) volume loss assessed by a qualitative rating using MRI). Subjects with other neurological or psychiatric diseases that could explain memory deficits were excluded, as well as subjects who received medication that could interfere with cognitive function. Basic characteristics of this sample are described in Table 1.

tab1
Table 1: Characterization of the study sample: ranges, mean, and standard deviations in brackets. MMSE: Mini-Mental Status Examination; WST: German Vocabulary Test (Wortschatztest) as a measure of Intelligence; CSF: cerebrospinal fluid; Low IQ ( ); High IQ ( ).
2.2. Neuropsychological Tests

General Intellectual Abilities
It has been shown that intelligence (i.e., reading ability) predicts incident dementia better than education measured in number of years [33]. As a measure of crystallized (general) intelligence we used a German vocabulary test (Wortschatztest, WST) [34]. Raw scores from the WST were converted into IQ scores, according to the manual. Patients were grouped according to general intellectual ability (low general intellectual ability with IQ   100 and high intellectual ability with IQ   100).

Dementia Severity
Dementia severity was staged with the MMSE [35]. In the present study, we included subjects with a MMSE score   22 because our goal was to describe gender differences in the early stages of AD and patients with more advanced stages of the disease are rarely able to complete the comprehensive neuropsychological tests applied in this study.

Episodic Memory
Verbal episodic memory with the immediate and delayed recall of a word list was tested with the German version of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) as previously applied [17, 36]. The word list is a shopping list whose items can be grouped into four categories (fruits, drinks, tools, clothes). This allows a cued recall, where subjects have to recall all items belonging to a specific category. As a measure of episodic memory (for immediate and delayed recall), the percent rate of correctly recalled words was used. Immediate and delayed recall of geometric forms was tested with the visual reproduction test (Wechsler Memory Scale, WMS) [37]. The percent rate of correctly recalled shapes is reported. In accordance with the Petersen criteria [32], the cut-off was set at 1.5 SD.

2.3. Analysis of Cerebrospinal Fluid

Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) routine parameters were analyzed. CSF for analysis of tau-protein was frozen immediately after lumbar puncture and stored at −80°C. CSF tau concentrations were determined at the University of Ulm using a sandwich ELISA (INNOTEST hTAU, Innogenetics) as described [38], which recognizes normally phosphorylated and unphosphorylated tau. The assay was adapted according to the protocol supplied with the kit (laboratory reference ranges: 200 pg/mL and 300 pg/mL for control individuals below 65 and older than 65 years, resp.), and tau concentrations of the samples were estimated from standard curves made for each assay. Tau levels 200 pg/mL (below 65 years) and 300 pg/mL (older than 65 years) were regarded as indicative of a neurodegenerative process. The analytical sensitivity of the assay was 75 pg/mL, and the intraassay and interassay variation was 8%. CSF Abeta-amyloid 1–42 levels were determined using a sandwich ELISA developed several years ago [39] and now commercially available (Innogenetics, Zwijndrecht, Belgium). The assay was performed according to the protocol supplied with the kit, and CSF Abeta concentrations of the samples were estimated from standard curves made for each assay. Abeta protein levels below 550 pg/mL were regarded as indicative of a neurodegenerative process.

2.4. Structural Neuroimaging

Structural cerebral imaging with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was classified by visual rating as representing normal findings or increased medio-temporal atrophy. A detailed quantitative regional analysis was beyond the scope of the present study.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Four groups were compared in this study: male and female AD patients with either high or low intellectual ability. In order to reduce alpha-error probability, statistical tests were applied in a hierarchical manner, that is, differences between 2 single groups were only investigated if the overall analysis of all four groups revealed significant differences. In order to provide comparability to other studies, data are presented as mean values with standard deviation (SD) and ranges. All statistical analyses were carried out using nonparametric testing (chi-square, Mann-Whitney-U-Test, Kruskal-Wallis test) applying the statistics program SPSS (SPSS 15.0 for Windows, Chicago, Ill, USA).

3. Results

The four groups were equal according to number of patients (N, Chi-square, ) and age (Kruskal-Wallis-Test, ) but differed in their MMSE scores (Kruskal-Wallis test, ). Men in the high IQ group had higher MMSE scores than men in the low IQ group (U-Test, ) and women in the high IQ group (U-Test, ).

The number of abnormal biomarkers (CSF tau, CSF Abeta, MRI) was not significantly different between the four groups (Kruskal-Wallis test, ). CSF biomarkers revealed similar levels for CSF Abeta-protein in all four groups (Kruskal-Wallis test, ) but differences in CSF tau levels (Kruskal-Wallis test, ). Women in the high IQ group had higher CSF tau values than men in the high IQ group (U-Test, ). Although women with higher IQ had numerically increased values for CSF tau than women with lower IQ, this difference reached no significance. CSF tau values were similar for women and men in low IQ patients.

The results of the episodic memory function for verbal and visuospatial material are presented in Table 2. Overall analysis revealed similar results for all four groups in episodic memory function for verbal material (Kruskal-Wallis test, ).

tab2
Table 2: Values for episodic memory function (in % of maximum score): means (SD).

However, episodic memory significantly differed between the four groups for visuospatial material (Kruskal-Wallice test, immediate recall: ; delayed recall: ). In delayed recall male AD patients achieved better test results than female patients (low IQ group: U-Test, ; high IQ group: U-Test, ). High IQ male patients showed better results than low IQ male patients in immediate recall (U-Test, ). In delayed recall, women with higher IQ performed better than women with lower IQ (U-Test, ).

4. Discussion

Recently, research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) have been proposed [5]. With this framework, diagnosis of AD is achieved on finding an episodic memory deficit plus AD-typical biological markers (e.g., CSF tau protein and Abeta-protein, atrophy on structural imaging). Considering the gender-specific profile of memory in AD patients [17] and modulation of memory in healthy adults by general intellectual abilities [40, 41], it was the goal to explore both, the influence of intelligence and gender on verbal and visuospatial memory in AD patients.

It has been proposed that CSF t-tau concentrations reflect the intensity or activity of the neurodegenerative process [42, 43]. This is supported by the respective CSF t-tau concentrations in various diseases with different progression rates (CJD   AD  > ALS  > MS). In addition, within a single disease entity, the CSF t-tau concentration likely indicates a faster progression rate [4446]. In AD, the tau levels in CSF correlate with the annual atrophy rate [47]. Abeta on the other hand has been proposed to give a measure of how far the disease process already has proceeded [42]. This is supported by the findings that the degree of atrophy measured on structural cerebral imaging [47] or Abeta deposition evaluated in vivo with positron emission tomography [48] correlate with CSF Abeta levels.

4.1. The Role of IQ

High functioning elders are particularly difficult to diagnose, as they score too high on standard tests for which the norms were established in populations of average general intellectual abilities [49]. Comparable to other studies in healthy adults [12, 40, 41], we found significant differences in cognitive function in AD patients depending on their premorbid IQ level. Male AD patients with higher IQ had better test results regarding their immediate memory for visuospatial material than male AD patients with lower IQ. CSF tau values (i.e., disease activity) were not different for male AD patients. These findings indicate a higher capacity for men with higher intelligence. Similar results were found in female patients for long delayed recall of visuospatial material. Although their CSF tau value was numerically (but not significantly) increased, women with higher IQ showed even better test results on their visuospatial episodic memory than women with lower IQ (see Figure 1), also indicating a higher capacity for women with higher intelligence.

638145.fig.001
Figure 1: Visuospatial episodic memory performance (bars indicate % of maximum score) in comparison to CSF tau values (lines) in male and female AD patients. Higher memory scores indicate better performance. Higher CSF tau values indicate more advanced disease (activity).
4.2. The Role of Gender

In the present study, we found gender-differences in episodic memory function for visuospatial material depending on IQ level. In the lower IQ group, men performed better than women. CSF tau (i.e., disease activity) values were similar in both groups. This result indicates that despite same levels of disease activity, male AD patients are better in remembering visuospatial material despite same levels of disease activity compared to female AD patients. In the higher IQ group, women also performed worse than men. CSF tau values for the high IQ group were increased for women, showing that disease activity was higher in this group. In a further analysis controlling for disease activity (CSF tau-value), gender differences in episodic memory function disappear, that is, no advantage for males was observed. We speculate that women with higher intellectual abilities might use strategies (e.g., verbalization of the material) which may explain the absence of gender differences for visuospatial material at least in the high IQ group. For future studies, material which is less subject to verbalization than even the present geometric patterns should be used.

Gender differences in the verbal domain that are well established for healthy controls [31] were not observed in the present study on patients with mild AD. Most likely, aging [28] and the disease process obscures gender specificities observed in healthy adult individuals.

5. Conclusion

Our results show that both IQ and gender modulate episodic memory in AD patients in a modality specific fashion (verbal versus visuospatial). Women with lower IQ appear to be particularly vulnerable to visuospatial episodic memory deficits. The dependence on gender and general intellectual capacity in episodic memory breakdown needs to be considered in the newly proposed research criteria for the diagnosis of early Alzheimer’s disease.

References

  1. D. L. Bachman, P. A. Wolf, R. Linn et al., “Prevalence of dementia and probable senile dementia of the Alzheimer type in the Framingham study,” Neurology, vol. 42, no. 1, pp. 115–119, 1992. View at Google Scholar
  2. R. Brookmeyer, S. Gray, and C. Kawas, “Projections of Alzheimer's disease in the United States and the public health impact of delaying disease onset,” American Journal of Public Health, vol. 88, no. 9, pp. 1337–1342, 1998. View at Google Scholar
  3. L. J. Launer, K. Andersen, M. E. Dewey et al., “Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer's disease: results from EURODEM pooled analyses. EURODEM Incidence Research Group and Work Groups. European Studies of Dementia,” Neurology, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 78–84, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  4. G. McKhann, D. Drachman, M. Folstein, R. Katzman, D. Price, and E. M. Stadlan, “Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease,” Neurology, vol. 34, no. 7, pp. 939–944, 1984. View at Google Scholar
  5. B. Dubois, H. H. Feldman, C. Jacova et al., “Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria,” Lancet Neurology, vol. 6, no. 8, pp. 734–746, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  6. T. Rabinowicz, D. E. Dean, J. M.-C. Petetot, and G. M. de Courten-Myers, “Gender differences in the human cerebral cortex: more neurons in males; more processes in females,” Journal of Child Neurology, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 98–107, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  7. K. P. Riley, D. A. Snowdon, and W. R. Markesbery, “Alzheimer's neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: findings from the Nun Study,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 51, no. 5, pp. 567–577, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  8. F. A. Schmitt, D. G. Davis, D. R. Wekstein, C. D. Smith, J. W. Ashford, and W. R. Markesbery, ““Preclinical” AD revisited: neuropathology of cognitively normal older adults,” Neurology, vol. 55, no. 3, pp. 370–376, 2000. View at Google Scholar
  9. D. J. Selkoe, “Alzheimer's disease is a synaptic failure,” Science, vol. 298, no. 5594, pp. 789–791, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  10. H. A. Lashuel, D. Hartley, B. M. Petre, T. Walz, and P. T. Lansbury Jr., “Neurodegenerative disease: amyloid pores from pathogenic mutations,” Nature, vol. 418, no. 6895, p. 291, 2002. View at Google Scholar
  11. Y. Stern, “What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 448–460, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  12. G. E. Alexander, M. L. Furey, C. L. Grady et al., “Association of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabolism in Alzheimer's disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 154, no. 2, pp. 165–172, 1997. View at Google Scholar
  13. Y. Stern, B. Gurland, T. K. Tatemichi, M. X. Tang, D. Wilder, and R. Mayeux, “Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 271, no. 13, pp. 1004–1010, 1994. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  14. Y. Stern, S. Albert, M.-X. Tang, and W.-Y. Tsai, “Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: cognitive reserve?” Neurology, vol. 53, no. 9, pp. 1942–1947, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  15. N. Scarmeas, G. Levy, M.-X. Tang, J. Manly, and Y. Stern, “Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease,” Neurology, vol. 57, no. 12, pp. 2236–2242, 2001. View at Google Scholar
  16. N. Le Carret, S. Auriacombe, L. Letenneur, V. Bergua, J.-F. Dartigues, and C. Fabrigoule, “Influence of education on the pattern of cognitive deterioration in AD patients: the cognitive reserve hypothesis,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 57, no. 2, pp. 120–126, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  17. U. Beinhoff, H. Tumani, J. Brettschneider, D. Bittner, and M. W. Riepe, “Gender-specificities in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment,” Journal of Neurology, vol. 255, no. 1, pp. 117–122, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  18. P. Alexopoulos, T. Grimmer, R. Perneczky, G. Domes, and A. Kurz, “Progression to dementia in clinical subtypes of mild cognitive impairment,” Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 27–34, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  19. A. Drzezga, N. Lautenschlager, H. Siebner et al., “Cerebral metabolic changes accompanying conversion of mild cognitive impairment into Alzheimer's disease: a PET follow-up study,” European Journal of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging, vol. 30, no. 8, pp. 1104–1113, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  20. A. Herlitz, L.-G. Nilsson, and L. Backman, “Gender differences in episodic memory,” Memory and Cognition, vol. 25, no. 6, pp. 801–811, 1997. View at Google Scholar
  21. A. Herlitz, E. Airaksinen, and E. Nordstrom, “Sex differences in episodic memory: the impact of verbal and visuospatial ability,” Neuropsychology, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 590–597, 1999. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  22. C. Lewin, G. Wolgers, and A. Herlitz, “Sex differences favoring women in verbal but not in visuospatial episodic memory,” Neuropsychology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 165–173, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  23. M. J. Aartsen, M. Martin, and D. Zimprich, “Gender differences in level and change in cognitive functioning: results from the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam,” Gerontology, vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 35–38, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  24. J. E. Yonker, E. Eriksson, L.-G. Nilsson, and A. Herlitz, “Sex differences in episodic memory: minimal influence of estradiol,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 52, no. 2, pp. 231–238, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  25. M. L. Bleecker, K. Bolla-Wison, J. Agnew, and D. A. Meyers, “Age-related sex differences in verbal memory,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 44, no. 3, pp. 403–411, 1988. View at Google Scholar
  26. E. Barrett-Connor and D. Kritz-Silverstein, “Gender differences in cognitive function with age: the Rancho Bernardo study,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 47, no. 2, pp. 159–164, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  27. J. H. Kramer, K. Yaffe, J. Lengenfelder, and D. C. Delis, “Age and gender interactions on verbal memory performance,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 97–102, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  28. S. B. Maitland, A. Herlitz, L. Nyberg, L. Backman, and L.-G. Nilsson, “Selective sex differences in declarative memory,” Memory and Cognition, vol. 32, no. 7, pp. 1160–1169, 2004. View at Google Scholar
  29. G. Grön, A. P. Wunderlich, M. Spitzer, R. Tomczak, and M. W. Riepe, “Brain activation during human navigation: gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 3, no. 4, pp. 404–408, 2000. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  30. T. D. Parsons, P. Larson, K. Kratz et al., “Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 42, no. 4, pp. 555–562, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  31. T. Fritsch, J. D. Larsen, and K. A. Smyth, “The role of adolescent IQ and gender in the use of cognitive support for remembering in aging,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 394–416, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  32. R. C. Petersen, R. Doody, A. Kurz et al., “Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 58, no. 12, pp. 1985–1992, 2001. View at Google Scholar
  33. B. Schmand, J. H. Smit, M. I. Geerlings, and J. Lindeboom, “The effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia. A test of the brain reserve hypothesis,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 27, no. 6, pp. 1337–1344, 1997. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  34. K. H. Schmidt and P. Metzler, WST—Wortschatztest, Beltz, Weinheim, Germany, 1992.
  35. M. F. Folstein, L. N. Robins, and J. E. Helzer, “The mini-mental state examination,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 40, no. 7, p. 812, 1983. View at Google Scholar
  36. G. Grön, D. Bittner, B. Schmitz, A. P. Wunderlich, and M. W. Riepe, “Subjective memory complaints: objective neural markers in patients with Alzheimer's disease and major depressive disorder,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 51, no. 4, pp. 491–498, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  37. D. Wechsler, “Wechsler memory scale—revised manual,” San Antonio, Tex, USA, 1987.
  38. M. Vandermeeren, M. Mercken, E. Vanmechelen et al., “Detection of tau proteins in normal and Alzheimer's disease cerebrospinal fluid with a sensitive sandwich enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay,” Journal of Neurochemistry, vol. 61, no. 5, pp. 1828–1834, 1993. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  39. R. Motter, C. Vigo-Pelfrey, D. Kholodenko et al., “Reduction of beta-amyloid peptide42 in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with Alzheimer's disease,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 643–648, 1995. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  40. L. J. Rapport, B. N. Axelrod, M. E. Theisen, D. B. Brines, A. D. Kalechstein, and J. H. Ricker, “Relationship of IQ to verbal learning and memory: test and retest,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 655–666, 1997. View at Google Scholar
  41. J. L. Woodard, F. C. Goldstein, V. J. Roberts, and C. McGuire, “Convergent and discriminant validity of the CVLT (dementia version). California Verbal Learning Test,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 553–558, 1999. View at Google Scholar
  42. K. Blennow, H. Zetterberg, L. Minthon et al., “Longitudinal stability of CSF biomarkers in Alzheimer's disease,” Neuroscience Letters, vol. 419, no. 1, pp. 18–22, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  43. K. Blennow and H. Hampel, “CSF markers for incipient Alzheimer's disease,” Lancet Neurology, vol. 2, no. 10, pp. 605–613, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  44. H. Arai, M. Terajima, M. Miura et al., “Tau in cerebrospinal fluid: a potential diagnostic marker in Alzheimer's disease,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 649–652, 1995. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  45. M. Riemenschneider, S. Wagenpfeil, H. Vanderstichele et al., “Phospho-tau/total tau ratio in cerebrospinal fluid discriminates Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease from other dementias,” Molecular Psychiatry, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 343–347, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  46. O. Hansson, H. Zetterberg, P. Buchhave, E. Londos, K. Blennow, and L. Minthon, “Association between CSF biomarkers and incipient Alzheimer's disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a follow-up study,” Lancet Neurology, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 228–234, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  47. L.-O. Wahlund and K. Blennow, “Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers for disease stage and intensity in cognitively impaired patients,” Neuroscience Letters, vol. 339, no. 2, pp. 99–102, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  48. A. M. Fagan, M. A. Mintun, R. H. Mach et al., “Inverse relation between in vivo amyloid imaging load and cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42 in humans,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 59, no. 3, pp. 512–519, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed
  49. D. M. Rents, T. J. Huh, L. M. Sardinha et al., “Intelligence quotient-adjusted memory impairment is associated with abnormal single photon emission computed tomography perfusion,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 821–831, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at PubMed