Abstract

Background. Globally, one in three adults has a chronic condition. Many chronic diseases that are not neurological in nature (e.g., diabetes and heart failure) are increasingly associated with cognitive symptoms. However, the instruments used to assess cognitive symptoms in those with nonneurologic chronic illness are heterogeneous, and questions remain as to how cognitive symptoms may be related to demographic and clinical outcome variables, neurocognitive test performance, and other patient-reported outcomes. In this review, we describe associations among self-reported cognitive function, cognitive performance, and additional patient-reported outcomes as well as how cognitive symptoms are measured in nonneurologic chronic illness. Method. Multiple databases (PubMed, Medline, CINAHL, PsycInfo, EMBASE, SCOPUS, the Cochrane Library, and Academic Search Complete) were searched for studies from 1990 to 2020 that provided data on self-reported cognitive symptoms in those with nonneurological chronic conditions. Initial search yielded 304 articles, of which 32 met inclusion criteria. Quality assessment was conducted using the Critical Appraisal Skills Programme. Results. Thirty-two total studies were included: twenty cross-sectional, 10 longitudinal, and 2 randomized controlled trials. The tools used to assess self-reported cognitive function in the studies were heterogeneous: 28 unique tools were used. Thirty studies examined associations among self-reported cognitive function and other patient-reported outcomes. In 19 there were significant associations. Six studies showed no significant associations between neuropsychological tests and self-reported cognitive function; another 6 studies found a significant association. Conclusion. Tools to assess cognitive symptoms were heterogeneous. In most studies, self-reported cognitive symptoms were not correlated with neuropsychological test results, but the majority of studies found a strong association between self-reported cognitive function and other patient-reported outcomes. Implications. Consensus on measuring cognitive symptoms would facilitate cross-study comparisons and facilitate scientific progress in those with nonneurological chronic conditions. Based on these results, there is a need to establish a standardized approach for self-reported cognitive function measurement in patients with nonneurologic chronic illness.

1. Introduction

National surveys suggest that more than 26% of older adults are concerned about a potential diagnosis of Alzheimer’s, and more than 50% are concerned about becoming a burden on family because of future cognitive problems [1, 2]. Some cognitive decline is expected in older adults, but cognitive changes that impair one’s ability to function in middle to late adulthood are unexpected. These changes are complex and multifaceted, especially in those with nonneurologic chronic conditions with known cognitive risk factors (e.g., diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer) [3, 4]. However, despite risk factors and the prevalence of cognitive changes in those with nonneurologic chronic conditions, less is known about cognitive function in such populations.

The effects and impact of cognitive dysfunction on day-to-day life such as difficulties in memory and deficits in attention are difficult to assess with standard neuropsychological tests [5]. Individuals’ perspectives are therefore critical to our understanding of cognitive symptoms, not only because perceived cognitive decline may be a precursor to mild cognitive development and dementia [6], but also because self-reported cognitive function captures the impact of cognitive symptoms on daily function. At least 20% of people 45 years and older with one chronic disease report having cognitive problems, and this prevalence may be higher for those with specific conditions [7]. Those who have had a stroke, a history of heart disease, or chronic obstructive pulmonary disease have a higher occurrence of self-reported cognitive symptoms than do those without those diseases [8]. For example, 27.1% of adults aged 45–65 years who have coronary artery disease report subjective cognitive problems, whereas in healthy adults 65 and older, the prevalence is 18.7% [7]. The presence of midlife self-reported cognitive dysfunction can be a risk for dementia, sometimes presenting before objective impairments are found with neuropsychological tests [9]. In addition, self-reported cognitive dysfunction can impact daily self-management of chronic conditions such as diabetes [10, 11] as well as quality of life [12, 13]. As a result, research on self-reported cognitive dysfunction in persons at risk for mild cognitive impairment has increased [14].

In this review, we describe associations among self-reported cognitive function (SRCF), cognitive performance, and additional patient-reported outcomes as well as how cognitive symptoms are measured in nonneurologic chronic illness.

2. Methods

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement, including the PRISMA 27-item checklist of essential review characteristics, informed the procedures for this review [15] (Supplementary Material A). The review protocol was registered with PROSPERO (CRD4202146706) per guidance from the Cochrane Collaborative [15].

2.1. Data Sources, Search Strategy, and Selection

The following databases were searched for articles related to SRCF in peer-reviewed journals from January 1990 through October 2020: CINAHL, MedLine, PubMed, PsycInfo, EMBASE, SCOPUS, the Cochrane Library, and Academic Search Complete.

Broad search terms (MeSH) and synonyms were used including subjective cognitive complaints, perceived cognitive problems, chronic conditions, and chronic disease (Supplementary Material B). Citations of all relevant studies were also reviewed. MedLine was searched first, and resulting syntax and headings were used to search the other databases. Key inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) use of a self-report measure of cognitive function (e.g., perceived cognitive issues, symptoms of cognitive problems, memory complaints); (2) participants 18 years of age or older; and (3) participants diagnosed with one or more nonneurologic chronic conditions (e.g., type 2 diabetes, coronary artery disease, and obstructive pulmonary disease). All quantitative study designs—randomized controlled trials (RCTs), cross-sectional studies, and longitudinal studies—were included. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) study participants diagnosed with neurologic chronic conditions, such as dementia, stroke, HIV-associated cognitive disorders, and central nervous system disorders; (2) publications that were not peer-reviewed or not written in English; and (3) posters, review papers, letters, and conference proceedings. We also excluded studies of those with chemotherapy-related cognitive dysfunction, because comprehensive reviews have examined self-reported cognitive function following chemotherapy treatment [16, 17]. All titles, abstracts, and full texts of the studies were independently screened by two reviewers, and disagreements were settled to ensure the studies’ eligibility.

2.2. Data Extraction

Data, extracted by all authors, included author, year of publication, research design, data collection time points, purpose of the study, study setting, sample characteristics, measures of SRCF and primary variables, and associations with objective neuropsychological tests. The first and second reviewers double-verified the extracted data for accuracy. When data needed for extraction were missing, the first author contacted the authors of the study via e-mail to request the data (Supplementary Material C).

2.3. Quality Appraisal

The Critical Appraisals Skills Programme (CASP) [18] was used to assess the quality of the included studies. In this review, randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were assessed using the CASP RCT checklist, and the remaining studies were evaluated using the CASP cohort study checklist.

2.4. Synthesis

Meta-analysis was not feasible, due to the variability among studies in design, measures of self-reported cognitive function, and outcome variables. The data were instead analyzed using Popay et al. [19] methods for narrative synthesis in systematic reviews.

3. Results

3.1. Search

Eight hundred and sixty-six eligible studies were retrieved from the databases. After duplicates (n = 562) were removed, 304 titles remained and were imported to an online platform from Covidence (https://covidence.org) for independent screening and data extraction. A total of 32 studies were included in the final analysis (See Figure 1 PRISMA flow diagram for details of screening.).

3.2. Risk of Bias/Quality Assessment

Two studies were RCTs; for both studies, all 11 items on the CASP [18] RCT checklist indicated relatively high study quality. Of the remaining studies, a few had high scores, with no quality issues recognized (n = 8) or one or two low (“No”) quality scores (n = 4). Most of the “No” scores were related to confounding factors (n = 3) and bias minimization (n = 1) (Supplementary Material D).

3.3. Characteristics of Included Studies

Of the 32 studies included in the analysis [2051], 20 (63%) were cross-sectional, 10 (31%) were longitudinal, and two (6%) were RCTs (Table 1). Of the two RCTs, one investigated the effects of a computerized cognitive training intervention in participants with chronic pain [23]. The other examined the effects of cognitive behavioral therapy on cognitive impairment, both objective and subjective [38]. Six studies compared participants with healthy controls [21, 25, 39, 42, 45, 49]. Four other studies used specific comparators: (1) treatment with steroids versus nontreatment [30]; (2) two or more chronic conditions versus one or none [35]; (3) amputation for vascular or nonvascular etiologies [41]; and (4) treatment with erythropoietin versus nontreatment [43]. Follow-up in the longitudinal studies ranged from 1 week to 2 years.

3.3.1. Study Populations

Sample sizes ranged from 26 to 11,379 with a mean age range of 30 to 78.5 years. The most common chronic conditions included coronary artery disease (10 studies), HIV (4 studies), and chronic pain (4 studies). Fifteen studies had a majority of females in the sample, 15 were majority males, and 2 were evenly split. Seventeen studies did not report the ethnic makeup of the samples. Sixteen of the studies were conducted in the U.S., and four were conducted in The Netherlands (See Table 1 and Supplementary Material E).

3.3.2. Self-Reported Cognitive Function Measures

Twenty-eight different tools were used to assess SRCF across the 32 studies. The most common were the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire (five studies) and the Cognitive Difficulties Scale (three studies). Other tools included the Everyday Memory Questionnaire, the Cognitive Complaints Inventory, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, and the Health Complaints Scale. Four were sets of author-derived questions, with items such as “Do you have any complaints concerning your memory?” (See Supplementary Material F). Three studies did not report the cognitive domains assessed by the SCRF measure [43, 47, 49] and two used “global” SCRF tools [30, 42]. The remaining studies ranged from measuring one domain (e.g., only executive function) [23, 39] to seven domains [26]. Ten studies [21, 25, 28, 31, 32, 36, 37, 44, 48, 51] reported reliability of the SCRF tools with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .67 to .8 for the Health Complaints Scale–Concentration subscale to .96 for the Cognitive Difficulties Scale. Items on each tool ranged from three [39] to 95 [21].

3.3.3. Reporting of Other Patient-Reported Outcomes

Thirty studies included assessment of other self-reported outcomes. The most common were the Beck Depression Inventory (6 studies) and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (3 studies). Thirteen studies (41%) included objective neuropsychological testing. The neuropsychological tests ranged from screening tests to computerized assessments to comprehensive batteries. Only one study included imaging [32].

3.4. Primary Study Results

Descriptions of study outcomes varied widely in design, SRCF related endpoints, and measures. Therefore, a meta-analysis could not be conducted. In some studies, only descriptive results of self-report measures were reported (e.g., means, percentages). Other studies gave more in-depth results including (1) comparison of symptoms of cognitive problems, usually between participants with a chronic illness and those who did not; (2) cognitive changes over time in longitudinal studies; and (3) differences in cognitive symptoms between groups in RCTs of interventions. Therefore, the number of studies was insufficient to conduct a meta-analysis.

3.4.1. Demographics and Self-Reported Cognitive Function

Three studies [33, 37, 48] focused mainly on participants’ demographic characteristics in relation to self-perceived cognitive function. One of these studies [37] investigated the relationship further, using longitudinal data collected at 12 months. Two of the three studies [33, 37] found a significant relationship between unemployment/early retirement/homemaker and increased cognitive complaints. Middle age (45–54 years) was also associated with more cognitive complaints [33], and older age (55 and older) was associated with more cognitive complaints [37, 48].

3.4.2. Self-Reported Cognitive Function and Neuropsychological Tests

Nine studies (28%) compared objective neuropsychological tests of cognitive function with SRCF [22, 24, 25, 27, 29, 32, 34, 42, 46] with Pearson’s r correlations ranging from −24 to −27 [25] to .44 to .85 [22]. Two of them found no significant relationships between self-report and objective measures [24, 27]. Four others [22, 25, 29, 32] identified relationships between specific cognitive domains and perceived function. In two of those four, memory concerns were significantly related to objective memory test performance [22, 32]. In another, greater overall perceived problems were associated with worse scores on executive function, processing speed, and language measurements [25]. The last found that higher scores on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment were significantly related to fewer everyday cognitive symptoms [29]. Steinbusch et al. [46] followed cognitive function over time and found that higher baseline cognitive complaints were significantly related to lower cognitive ability at 12 months. Jackson and Cooper [34] found that a diagnosis of hypertension was associated with worse objective cognitive function over time in those with SRCF than in those without. Similarly, Nguyen et al. [42] showed that community dwelling older adults who had hypertension and memory concerns had worse objective performance on cognitive tests than did nonhypertensive participants with memory complaints.

3.4.3. Other Patient-Reported Outcomes and SRCF

A number of additional patient-reported outcomes were associated with SRCF in samples with various nonneurologic chronic conditions. Two studies reported findings from separate samples undergoing interventions for cardiovascular disease—percutaneous coronary interventions (PCIs) and coronary artery bypass surgery (CABG) [28, 31]. For those undergoing PCI, poorer perceived cognitive function was associated with poorer quality of life independently of demographics, fatigue, mood, and other clinical variables [28]. In the sample undergoing CABG, baseline cognitive complaints predicted a higher rate of negative emotional symptoms at 5 months [31]. For those with cardiovascular disease, but not undergoing any cardiac interventions, worse SRCF had a significant association with worse quality of life [36]. For patients with chronic pain, female gender, pain intensity, catastrophizing, posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, location of pain, and fatigue were positively associated with cognitive complaints. Depression and fatigue were most predictive [40, 44]. Depression severity and worse work functioning were significantly associated with poorer SRCF in depressed patients [20]. For rheumatoid arthritis patients, sleep quality was significantly associated with SRCF [50]. Zhu, Hu, Xing, Guo, and Wu [51] reported that increased levels of HIV-related discrimination were associated with higher levels of SCRD even after controlling for demographics, mental health conditions, and social support.

3.4.4. Severity of Chronic Conditions and Self-Reported Cognitive Function

Eleven studies (34%) examined associations between severity of chronic conditions and SRCF [21, 26, 3235, 39, 41, 46, 47, 49]. Overall, multimorbidity and higher severity of disease were positively associated with greater self-reported cognitive problems. However, in one of these studies [39], those with type 2 diabetes mellitus (T2DM) and diagnosed cognitive impairment did not differ in the number of cognitive complaints when compared with those with T2DM who were cognitively healthy.

4. Discussion

Almost every evaluation of self-reported cognitive symptoms used a unique approach to assess self-reported cognitive function (28 of 32 studies, 88%). This heterogeneity of instrumentation can inhibit data sharing and generalizability of results across diverse populations. The use of common data elements for self-reported cognitive function in persons with nonneurologic chronic illness could contribute to accelerating intervention development and testing [13]. The reviewed studies used 28 measures to assess SRCF, with little overlap among them. A prior review and meta-analysis of studies (n = 53; 20,319 participants) examined the association between objective and subjective cognitive function in normatively aging adults without chronic illnesses and found that self-reported cognitive function accounted for less than 1% of the performance in objective measures [52]. However, that review included studies that used five specific measures for subjective memory and likely excluded a large number of other studies, because there are not any “gold-standard” assessments of self-reported cognitive function. We therefore expanded on that review in two ways: by focusing on self-reported cognitive function and a broader population, adults with at least one chronic condition. Two other meta-analyses of self-reported memory concerns and prediction of mild cognitive impairment and dementia (n = 49 studies) showed that conversion to dementia was 1.5 to 3 times higher in those who had self-reported cognitive complaints than in those who did not [53, 54]. Both reviews also noted the lack of established/standardized measures for self-reported cognitive function and the impact of depression on cognitive symptoms.

In the studies in this review, increased severity of chronic disease was associated with greater subjective cognitive impairment. Chronic diseases are themselves associated with a number of negative consequences such as lower quality of life, increased mortality, and loss of independence [55, 56]. A number of mechanisms dependent on the type of chronic illness may be responsible for this association. For example, in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, low oxygen levels may directly affect the brain [57]. Or, more generally, physical illness can lead to fatigue and the subjective feeling of “not thinking well” [58]. It may also be that increased severity of disease has led to a decrease in leisure activities, exercise, sleep, or functional independence, which have protective effects on cognition [59]. Additionally, it has been shown that subjective cognitive dysfunction can impact daily self-management of chronic conditions like diabetes [10, 11] and quality of life [13, 59]. For these reasons, qualitative and quantitative research on subjective cognitive dysfunction in persons at risk for dementia are needed.

5. Conclusions

As this review demonstrates, many tools are used to measure self-reported cognitive symptoms, and clinicians should be aware that instrument selection will likely impact results. However, other reviews have found that self-reported cognitive complaints are a valid indicator of cognitive decline [60, 61]. It may be that deciding on a high-quality psychometric tool and using it consistently are important for clinical practice. Confounders for cognitive symptoms may also need to be assessed in clinical settings; anxiety, neuroticism, and dementia-related worry are variables related to increased subjective cognitive symptoms [60, 62].

Jessen et al. [63] provide a framework for investigating subjective cognitive symptoms in clinical settings and research. This framework includes suggested variables to examine (e.g., onset of subjective cognitive decline, believing one’s cognitive performance is worse than of those of the same age) and criteria that increase the possibility of the existence of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease in people with subjective cognitive decline. Molinuevo et al. [64] also suggest differentiating between “complaints” and “worries,” using a measure appropriate for the target population and including measures of stress, depression, and anxiety. Although the criteria in these two studies, from the Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative Working Group, indicate that nonneurologic medical issues (e.g., chronic conditions) could underlie self-reported cognitive decline due to poor physical health, the studies do not suggest any recommendation other than to use care in interpreting the results of subjective cognitive complaints. Longitudinal assessment of subjective cognitive complaints may also be important, because changes in SRCF can indicate the functional benefit of prevention interventions.

5.1. Limitations

This review contributes a synthesis of measures and characteristics of self-reported cognitive symptoms in persons with nonneurologic chronic illnesses, but the heterogeneity of studies’ effect sizes, outcomes, and measures did not permit data pooling, limiting cross study. The wide range of tests to measure perceived cognitive function likely contributed to variation in findings.

Cognitive impairment and chronic illness are both prevalent and detrimental. Given the present review’s heterogeneity in assessment tools and evidence limitations, it is possible that self-management of chronic illness is influenced by the cooccurrence of cognitive symptoms. Future prospective longitudinal studies should examine the relationship of perceived cognitive symptoms and the self-management of chronic conditions, and assessments and interventions for improving cognitive function should be incorporated into care for older adults with chronic conditions.

Data Availability

The data are available in the submitted supplementary files.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors do not have any conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

Editorial support was provided by Dr. John Bellquist at the Cain Center for Nursing Research and the Transdisciplinary Precision Health Intervention Methodology Training Program (PI Kim: T32 NR01903520) at The University of Texas at Austin School of Nursing.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials. Supplementary Material A. Methods. Supplementary Material B. Search Strategy. Supplementary Material C. Data Extraction. Supplementary Material D. Quality Appraisals. Supplementary Material E. Study Demographics. Supplementary Material F. Table of Subjective Cognitive Function Measures. (Supplementary Materials)