Table of Contents Author Guidelines Submit a Manuscript
Journal of Ophthalmology
Volume 2015, Article ID 607635, 8 pages
http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2015/607635
Research Article

Attenuating Photostress and Glare Disability in Pseudophakic Patients through the Addition of a Short-Wave Absorbing Filter

Vision Sciences, Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA

Received 16 December 2014; Revised 19 February 2015; Accepted 25 February 2015

Academic Editor: Antonio Benito

Copyright © 2015 Billy R. Hammond. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

To evaluate the effects of filtering short wavelength light on visual performance under intense light conditions among pseudophakic patients previously implanted with a clear intraocular lens (IOL). This was a patient-masked, randomized crossover study conducted at 6 clinical sites in the United States between September 2013 and January 2014. One hundred fifty-four bilaterally pseudophakic patients were recruited. Photostress recovery time and glare disability thresholds were measured with clip-on blue-light-filtering and placebo (clear; no blue-light filtration) glasses worn over patients’ habitual correction. Photostress recovery time was quantified as the time necessary to regain sight of a grating target after intense light exposure. Glare disability threshold was assessed as the intensity of a white-light annulus necessary to obscure a central target. The order of filter used and test eye were randomized across patients. Photostress recovery time and glare disability thresholds were significantly improved (both ) when patients used blue-light-filtering glasses compared with clear, nonfiltering glasses. Compared with a nonfiltering placebo, adding a clip-on blue-absorbing filter to the glasses of pseudophakic patients implanted with clear IOLs significantly increased their ability to cope with glare and to recover normal viewing after an intensive photostress. This result implies that IOL designs with blue-light-filtering characteristics may be beneficial under intense light conditions.

1. Introduction

The transmission of light energy drastically increases following cataract extraction in the aphakic or pseudophakic eye. Conventional intraocular lenses (IOLs) tend to block only ultraviolet light wavelengths (≤400 nm), but unlike the natural crystalline lens, they do not block light in the short-wave visible region of the spectrum [1] commonly referred to as the blue-light spectrum. Blue-light-filtering (BLF) IOLs were originally designed to reduce this increase in actinic light exposure. A large body of data [2] dating back to Ham’s original studies in 1976 [3] suggests that visible blue light is associated with special risks (e.g., photooxidative damage) [4, 5] when the blue-light blocking normally provided by anterior structures of the eye is lost. Without natural blue-light-blocking mechanisms, the blue light that reaches the retina is sufficiently energetic to initiate oxidative damage, and the retina/retina pigment epithelium complex contains high amounts of photosensitizers (e.g., lipofuscin) with an action spectrum that matches the waveband of approximately 400 to 500 nm (hence, the description of the “blue-light hazard”).

Another ramification of filtering light in the visible spectrum is that it alters the incoming stimulus and changes visual function, as has been demonstrated by studies using psychophysical methods to measure the optical density of naturally occurring intraocular BLFs like the anterior lens [6] or macular pigment [7]. Such filtering has a practical advantage in improving vision in photopic conditions in a variety of species, including humans [8, 9]. For example, Wooten and Hammond [10] originally argued that BLF could influence visual range (how far one can see outdoors) by selectively attenuating the deleterious effects of atmospheric blue haze (see the empirical validation of the original modeling by Hammond and colleagues [11]).

Blue-light filtering also reduces the deleterious effects of intense light. Manufacturers of spectacle lenses or sunglasses designed for this purpose face the challenge of reducing glare without reducing visibility. This is often accomplished by using strategies that mimic biological mechanisms for adapting to variations in light intensity. For example, photochromic lenses becoming darker in proportion to light intensity are analogous to how the visual system adjusts sensitivity. Polarizing lenses absorb horizontally oriented glare and pass vertical orientations that tend to not produce glare. Filtering shorter wavelengths tends to also spare visibility since blue light is off the peak of the photopic spectrum. Hence, BLFs reduce retinal exposure to the wavelengths that tend to be the most actinic, photophobic [12], and susceptible to atmospheric (i.e., Rayleigh) scatter while minimally absorbing the medium- to long-wave light that mediates object perception [13].

One assumption inherent in many BLF IOL studies is that the filtering itself is one of the primary factors that produce the visual improvements observed with blue-light-absorbing IOLs. The current study was designed to examine the isolated effects of blue-light filtering. This was done by measuring photostress recovery and glare disability thresholds in pseudophakic patients implanted with clear (i.e., non-BLF) IOLs. A clip-on spectacle lens with filtering characteristics matched to a commonly used BLF IOL (the AcrySof Natural IOL; Alcon Laboratories, Inc.) was then compared to a matched clear lens in a within-patient, same-eye crossover design (see transmission spectra [1, 14]).

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design and Patients

This was a prospective, randomized, patient-masked crossover study conducted at 6 clinical sites in the United States from September 2014 to January 2014 (This trial is registered with Clinicaltrials.gov NCT01938989). The study consisted of a single visit.

The participating clinicians (listed in the acknowledgments) preselected candidates who fit several criteria. Eligible patients were bilaterally pseudophakic and ≥3 months postimplantation with clear IOLs. Patients were required to be ≥21 years of age, be in good ocular health (based on a clinical interview), and be able to adequately participate in the psychophysical testing. Patients with ocular pathology, degeneration, or media opacity that could have affected study assessments were excluded. Slit-lamp examinations were used to confirm the presence of clear ocular media and the absence of clinically significant posterior capsule opacification. Patients were excluded from participation if they had any conditions that could be exacerbated, triggered, or worsened by exposure to high-intensity light.

Patients were randomized to the order of use of BLF and non-BLF (clear) clip-on glasses, which were worn over patients’ habitual correction. Equivalence of the transmission spectrum of the BLF glasses with the natural chromophore used in a commercially available IOL (AcrySof Natural; Alcon Laboratories, USA) was confirmed before use in the study. The BLF and non-BLF clip-on glasses at each study site were reused for all patients evaluated at that site. Patients were masked to the identity of the test and control clip-on glasses. One eye per patient was randomly selected for testing.

The primary efficacy measure was photostress recovery time with the BLF versus non-BLF clip-on glasses. Supportive efficacy measures included glare disability threshold, monocular corrected visual acuity as assessed using the 100% contrast ETDRS chart under photopic lighting, and pupil size. Baseline measures included manifest refraction and uncorrected and corrected distance visual acuity.

The study protocol was approved by an independent ethics committee and Institutional Review Board (Aspire IRB; Santee, CA, USA) for all participating clinical sites. The experimental procedures adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Patients were informed about the aims and methods of the study, and all patients signed a statement of informed consent.

2.2. Equipment and Procedures

The apparatus used to measure glare disability and photostress recovery time was a 2-channel Maxwellian view system and is shown in Figure 1. The apparatus used white LEDs (LXML-PWN2, 4100 K; Phillips LumiLeds Lighting Company, San Jose, CA, USA) that provided a relatively broad spectrum of energy (major peaks at 445 and 570 nm) as the glare source.

Figure 1: Schematic of the optical system used to measure disability glare thresholds and photostress recovery time. A1-A2: apertures; BS: beam splitters; D1-D2: diffusers; EC: final focusing lens and eye cup for head positioning; FH: filter holder; G: grating and aperture for defining target; L1–L3: planoconvex achromatic lenses; M: mirror; PC: photocell; S1-S2: LED light sources.

The intensity of this glare source was electronically controlled (via pulse-width modulation) with customized computer software. One channel presented a target which was a 1°-diameter disk (peak wavelength, 570 nm) containing a contrast grating stimulus (4 cycles per degree). The luminance of the bars within the grating was 0.1 candela/m2. This target stimulus was used for both the glare disability and photostress measurements. To aid patients in maintaining the same position during the procedure, an eyepiece with a soft rubber eye cup was used. Prior to testing, patients were aligned to the optical system (this was facilitated by the structure of the eye cup). The BLF- and non-BLF-absorbing filters that we tested were incorporated into the eyepiece so that they were not visible to the patient. Careful adjustments were made such that the image was in focus and in the plane of the patient’s pupil.

When testing photostress recovery, the 1° target stimulus was shuttered at 500 ms on and off and a second channel provided a photobleaching light of high intensity (corneal irradiance, 5 log Trolands). The photobleaching light was presented for 5 seconds. The patients were informed before the photostressor appeared and were instructed to keep their eyes open for the duration of the exposure. At the end of the 5-second exposure, timing began to determine the length of time required until the target stimulus became visible again. At the point of reemergence, the participant pressed a button and the timing stopped. There were 3 repetitions separated by a waiting period of ≥2 minutes for each patient.

When testing glare disability, the target stimulus was presented at 2 seconds on and 1 second off. A second channel provided an annulus with an 11-degree inner diameter and 12-degree outer diameter. Before each trial, the annulus was set at a level well below that which would cause the target stimulus to be veiled. The intensity of the annulus was then adjusted by the experimenter until the patient indicated verbally that the target stimulus was no longer visible. This procedure was repeated for a total of 5 measurements (recorded as time in seconds), and patients were instructed to maintain their criterion threshold across trials. Five measurements were planned to be recorded per eye, unless a patient’s values had more than ~5% variability among measurements, in which case up to 4 additional measurements were conducted.

Careful calibrations were conducted in this study to ensure that the stimuli did not vary across sites. Hence, both radiometric and photometric calibrations were regularly performed. Prior to each experimental sitting, a dedicated radiometer was used to ensure that total light output remained constant (S370 Optometer with a PIN-10 photohead, UDT Instruments, Hawthorne, CA, USA). Photometric calibrations were done using a telescopic spectral radiometer (model PR650, PhotoResearch Inc. Chatsworth, CA, USA) with the stimuli projected onto a white reflectance standard calibrated to the instrument. Spatial alignment of the channels was checked every session by increasing the intensity of the light source and checking the precise location of the projected image against a fixed plate on the optical table.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Endpoints were analyzed in the efficacy analysis data set, which consisted of patients who provided data on ≥1 of the efficacy endpoints.

The arithmetic mean photostress recovery time with BLF versus non-BLF glasses was compared using a 1-sided paired -test. The difference between arithmetic means, 1-sided 95% CI, and values were calculated. The arithmetic mean of glare disability threshold () for each patient was transformed by solving for in the equation . The resulting values were log10 transformed. Glare disability threshold data were compared for BLF versus non-BLF glasses using paired -tests. Corrected visual acuity, pupil size, and demographic/baseline characteristic data were summarized descriptively.

Assuming a log-transformed photostress recovery time SD of 0.35, a minimum sample size of 153 patients was determined to provide 80% power to detect a 20% difference in photostress recovery time.

3. Results

One hundred fifty-four of 156 enrolled patients completed the study (97.5%). One patient was invalidated because they violated inclusion/exclusion criteria (implantation with a BLF IOL). Nine other patients had incomplete data sets due to physical limitations or inability to maintain alignment with the optical system. Most patients were white (94.9%) and there were more women (58.3%) than men (41.7%). Details regarding the patients tested in this study are listed in Table 1.

Table 1: Patient demographics and baseline characteristics.

Mean photostress recovery time was significantly lower when patients were wearing BLF compared to non-BLF clip-on glasses (; 95% CI, −2.08 to −0.66 seconds; Table 2). The mean ± SD difference between BLF and non-BLF glasses was seconds. Glare disability thresholds were significantly higher (i.e., patients could tolerate more light before losing sight of the central grating target) with BLF versus non-BLF glasses (; 95% CI, 0.06–0.18). The difference between BLF and non-BLF glasses was log units.

Table 2: Photostress recovery time and glare disability threshold (efficacy analysis data set).

Corrected visual acuity was comparable with versus without BLF. Mean ± SD acuity was logMAR with the BLF clip-on glasses (range: −0.30 to 0.34 logMAR) and logMAR with the non-BLF clip-on glasses (range: −0.3 to 0.38 logMAR). Mean ± SD pupil size was  mm and  mm with the BLF and non-BLF clip-on glasses, respectively.

4. Discussion

This study was aimed at assessing the role of additional blue filtration on vision when assessed under intense light conditions. Patients with pseudophakia with clear implants had a short-wave filter placed in front of their eyes which had absorption characteristics matching the young natural crystalline lens [1, 14]. This filtering lens was placed behind optical baffling and an eye cup so that patients would remain blinded to the comparison with a lens transparent to visible light. The order of testing conditions was randomized. Consistent with past studies (see Table 3) that have compared clear and BLF IOLs, we found a visual advantage of BLFs when testing glare disability and photostress recovery. Blue-light filtration increased tolerance to veiling white light and significantly lowered glare disability thresholds. For example, the amount of energy (i.e., light intensity) required to veil the central target in the glare disability assessment was about 23% higher when using the BLF lens compared to the clear lens (this percentage is based on a linear translation of the logged values). Photostress recovery time was improved by about 28%. These changes could translate to meaningful differences in everyday life. As a practical example, if someone is driving 60 mph and is blinded by the sun or bright headlights, a 10-second photostress recovery time translates to about 880 feet traveled before normal visual function is recovered. Regaining visual function 28% more quickly means seeing about 246 feet sooner (about two thirds the length of a football field).

Table 3: Studies on blue-light intraocular filters and visual benefit.

The mechanism for how blue-light filtering reduces photostress is straightforward: it simply reduces the intensity of the exposure and, hence, decreases recovery time. Photostress is caused by short intense light exposure (which is particularly damaging when dark adapted due to higher levels of photosensitizing photopigment [4]). Such exposure causes adaptive change and photopigment isomerization which results in temporary loss of vision. Endogenous BLF mechanisms such as the natural crystalline lens or macular pigment and artificial mechanisms such as BLF IOLs can absorb the incoming light and the forward scatter reducing the resultant loss of vision. This filtering protection, which operates over short exposures to intense light, does not lower overall sensitivity since the visual system adjusts sensitivity to offset stable changes in illumination [39, 40].

The mechanism for glare disability is less straightforward. It has been argued [41, 42], for instance, that intraocular filters (macular pigment or BLF IOLs) do not reduce glare disability. The basis for this argument is ecological; to wit, the authors note that the empirical studies that have found that BLF IOLs or macular pigment reduce glare disability have done so because they are “…biased by using bluer glare than target illumination, guaranteeing that glare light is preferentially reduced by yellow chromophores. Headlights and sunlight cause glare and illuminate targets. That’s why clinical glare tests use glare and target illumination with similar spectra simulating real-world conditions” [42].

The question is whether similar spectra really simulate real-world conditions; we would argue that they do not. Unless a target (an object in the patient’s line of sight) reflects all wavelengths equally (unlikely unless it is a mirrored surface or a perfect white), it would never have the same spectrum as a glare source such as the sun. Perhaps a more reasonable framing would be whether it is likely that a glare source (like the sun) would have more short-wave energy when compared to a probable target (hence, favoring a yellow intraocular filter). This is likely for several reasons.(1)The most common source of glare is the white light of the sun, which has a strong short-wave component [43].(2)Glare sources often come in from the side or above, whereas targets are, by definition, objects that are within our line of sight. Hence, even under the highly unlikely circumstance that the glare source and target share the exact same spectra, the composition would not be the same at the plane of the retina. This is because of the differences in light path [10]. Light reflected from an object within our line of sight passes through the atmosphere and short-wave energy scatters out of the light path (Rayleigh scatter does the opposite for objects in the surround).(3)Target stimuli are often composed of medium-wave light. One argument for the evolution of spectral sensitivity that peaks in the medium-wave region of the visible spectrum is that such a spectrum matches objects that are commonly perceived in the environment [44]. Many of the major pigments throughout nature are represented in the medium-and-longer-wave region. For example, chlorophyll is green and most of the carotenoids are yellow, orange, and red (few pigments one sees in a natural landscape are blue).(4)The light stimulus for S-cones (i.e., blue-light-sensing cones), is largely filtered by yellow intraocular filters, which are relatively sparse, contribute little to the luminosity function, and mostly mediate color perception [45]. Spatial vision is largely mediated by medium- and long-wave cones (this spectra tuned to match ecological condition).

Based on these reasons, it could be argued that many of the available clinical glare tests that match the spectra of the glare source and target are not ecologically valid (rather, they are designed to diagnose clinical conditions). The best way to measure glare disability is to use a glare source that matches a source that is commonly encountered, such as the sun, and to use a target that is strongly at the peak of the photopic spectral sensitivity curve, thereby matching the spectral content of most objects one would view.

Several empirical studies have examined the effects of macular pigment and BLF IOLs on visual function under glare conditions (see Table 3).

Most of the BLF IOL studies have used a case-control design, with the exception of Hammond et al. (2010) [18], which compared glare disability and photostress recovery using a contralateral design where visual function was tested in 1 eye implanted with a BLF IOL compared to the other eye with a clear IOL. That study found significant visual benefit in the eye with the blue-light-filtering IOL. Hammond and colleagues [17, 18] and Gray and colleagues [15, 16] used stimuli or test circumstances that closely match real-world scenarios; in the study by Hammond, visual stimuli were matched to daytime sunlight, and Gray tested patients using a driving simulator. In these studies, there was a clear benefit of the BLF IOLs with regard to photostress recovery and glare disability thresholds, similar to the findings of the current study. The results of other studies are largely mixed. Niwa et al. [22] and Pandita et al. [23] reported significant improvements using a BLF IOL when contrast sensitivity was measured under glare conditions, whereas K. Hayashi and H. Hayashi [19], Muftuoglu et al. [20], and Neumaier-Ammerer et al. [21] found no difference in glare disability threshold or contrast sensitivity function between clear and BLF IOLs. However, these studies used light sources without a significant blue-light component and/or glare devices without strong discriminative ability [38].

The use of clip-on glasses worn over pseudophakic patients’ habitual correction is a potential limitation of this study. Additionally, a single light source was used and psychophysical testing was not performed under varied light conditions (e.g., mesopic and scotopic).

In this study, photostress recovery time and glare disability thresholds were significantly reduced by blue-light filtration, whereas visual acuity was not compromised. Our findings are consistent with the visual benefit one might predict from simply returning the eye closer to its natural state, in this case, the young natural crystalline lens. Yellow (i.e., blue-light filtering) chromophores added to an IOL may help ameliorate complications of photostress and glare disability.

Conflict of Interests

Billy R. Hammond has received speaker fees from Alcon Research, Ltd.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by Alcon Research, Ltd. Study performance, data analysis, and paper preparation were conducted in accordance with GPP2 guidelines. Emily Bovier, Ph.D., Michael Engles, Ph.D., and Laura Fletcher, M.S., assisted in data collection. Kevin O’Brien, M.S., was involved in equipment design. Jay Pepose, M.D., Jeff Whittsett, M.D., Bret Fisher, M.D., John Berdahl, M.D., Cynthia Matossian, M.D., and Robert Rivera, M.D., supervised the collaborating clinical sites and conducted the clinical assessments.

References

  1. P. H. Ernest, “Light-transmission-spectrum comparison of foldable intraocular lenses,” Journal of Cataract & Refractive Surgery, vol. 30, no. 8, pp. 1755–1758, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  2. D. T. Organisciak and D. K. Vaughan, “Retinal light damage: mechanisms and protection,” Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 113–134, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  3. W. T. Ham Jr., H. A. Mueller, and D. H. Sliney, “Retinal sensitivity to damage from short wavelength light,” Nature, vol. 260, no. 5547, pp. 153–155, 1976. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  4. B. R. Hammond, B. A. Johnson, and E. R. George, “Oxidative photodegradation of ocular tissues: beneficial effects of filtering and exogenous antioxidants,” Experimental Eye Research, vol. 129, pp. 135–150, 2014. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  5. C. Delcourt, A. Cougnard-Grégoire, M. Boniol et al., “Lifetime exposure to ambient ultraviolet radiation and the risk for cataract extraction and age-related macular degeneration: the Alienor Study,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, vol. 55, no. 11, pp. 7619–7627, 2014. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  6. B. R. Wooten, B. R. Hammond, and L. M. Renzi, “Using scotopic and photopic flicker to measure lens optical density,” Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 321–328, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  7. B. R. Hammond Jr., B. R. Wooten, and B. Smollon, “Assessment of the validity of in vivo methods of measuring human macular pigment optical density,” Optometry and Vision Science, vol. 82, no. 5, pp. 387–404, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  8. B. R. Hammond Jr. and L. M. Fletcher, “Influence of the dietary carotenoids lutein and zeaxanthin on visual performance: application to baseball,” The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, vol. 96, no. 5, pp. 1207S–1213S, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  9. G. L. Walls and H. D. Judd, “The intra-ocular colour-filters of vertebrates,” British Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 17, pp. 641–675, 1933. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  10. B. R. Wooten and B. R. Hammond, “Macular pigment: influences on visual acuity and visibility,” Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 225–240, 2002. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  11. B. R. Hammond, B. R. Wooten, M. Engles, and J. C. Wong, “The influence of filtering by the macular carotenoids on contrast sensitivity measured under simulated blue haze conditions,” Vision Research, vol. 63, pp. 58–62, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  12. J. M. Stringham, K. Fuld, and A. J. Wenzel, “Action spectrum for photophobia,” Journal of the Optical Society of America A, vol. 20, no. 10, pp. 1852–1858, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  13. B. R. Hammond Jr., “The visual effects of intraocular colored filters,” Scientifica, vol. 2012, Article ID 424965, 18 pages, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  14. M. Tanito, T. Okuno, Y. Ishiba, and A. Ohira, “Transmission spectrums and retinal blue-light irradiance values of untinted and yellow-tinted intraocular lenses,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 299–307, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  15. R. Gray, S. A. Perkins, R. Suryakumar, B. Neuman, and W. A. Maxwell, “Reduced effect of glare disability on driving performance in patients with blue light-filtering intraocular lenses,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 37, no. 1, pp. 38–44, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  16. R. Gray, W. Hill, B. Neuman, D. Houtman, and R. Potvin, “Effects of a blue light-filtering intraocular lens on driving safety in glare conditions,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 38, no. 5, pp. 816–822, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  17. B. R. Hammond, B. Bernstein, and J. Dong, “The effect of the AcrySof natural lens on glare disability and photostress,” American Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 148, no. 2, pp. 272.e2–276.e2, 2009. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  18. B. R. Hammond Jr., L. M. Renzi, S. Sachak, and S. F. Brint, “Contralateral comparison of blue-filtering and non-blue-filtering intraocular lenses: glare disability, heterochromatic contrast, and photostress recovery,” Clinical Ophthalmology, vol. 4, no. 1, pp. 1465–1473, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  19. K. Hayashi and H. Hayashi, “Visual function in patients with yellow tinted intraocular lenses compared with vision in patients with non-tinted intraocular lenses,” British Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 90, no. 8, pp. 1019–1023, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  20. O. Muftuoglu, F. Karel, and R. Duman, “Effect of a yellow intraocular lens on scotopic vision, glare disability, and blue color perception,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 658–666, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  21. B. Neumaier-Ammerer, S. Felke, S. Hagen et al., “Comparison of visual performance with blue light-filtering and ultraviolet light-filtering intraocular lenses,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 36, no. 12, pp. 2073–2079, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  22. K. Niwa, Y. Yoshino, F. Okuyama, and T. Tokoro, “Effects of tinted intraocular lens on contrast sensitivity,” Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, vol. 16, no. 4, pp. 297–302, 1996. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  23. D. Pandita, S. M. Raj, V. A. Vasavada, N. S. Kazi, and A. R. Vasavada, “Contrast sensitivity and glare disability after implantation of AcrySof IQ Natural aspherical intraocular lens: prospective randomized masked clinical trial,” Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 603–610, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  24. B. R. Hammond Jr., L. M. Fletcher, and J. G. Elliott, “Glare disability, photostress recovery, and chromatic contrast: relation to macular pigment and serum lutein and zeaxanthin,” Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 476–481, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  25. B. R. Hammond, L. M. Fletcher, F. Roos, J. Wittwer, and W. Schalch, “A double-blind, placebo-controlled study on the effects of lutein and zeaxanthin on photostress recovery, glare disability, and chromatic contrast,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, vol. 55, no. 12, pp. 8583–8589, 2014. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  26. J. Kvansakul, M. Rodriguez-Carmona, D. F. Edgar et al., “Supplementation with the carotenoids lutein or zeaxanthin improves human visual performance,” Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, vol. 26, no. 4, pp. 362–371, 2006. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  27. J. Loughman, J. M. Nolan, A. N. Howard, E. Connolly, K. Meagher, and S. Beatty, “The impact of macular pigment augmentation on visual performance using different carotenoid formulations,” Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, vol. 53, no. 12, pp. 7871–7880, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  28. J. Loughman, M. C. Akkali, S. Beatty et al., “The relationship between macular pigment and visual performance,” Vision Research, vol. 50, no. 13, pp. 1249–1256, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  29. J. M. Nolan, J. Loughman, M. C. Akkali et al., “The impact of macular pigment augmentation on visual performance in normal subjects: COMPASS,” Vision Research, vol. 51, no. 5, pp. 459–469, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  30. B. Olmedilla, F. Granado, I. Blanco, and M. Vaquero, “Lutein, but not α-tocopherol, supplementation improves visual function in patients with age-related cataracts: a 2-y double-blind, placebo-controlled Pilot Study,” Nutrition, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 21–24, 2003. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  31. L. M. Renzi and B. R. Hammond, “The effect of macular pigment on heterochromatic luminance contrast,” Experimental Eye Research, vol. 91, no. 6, pp. 896–900, 2010. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  32. S. Richer, W. Stiles, L. Statkute et al., “Double-masked, placebo-controlled, randomized trial of lutein and antioxidant supplementation in the intervention of atrophic age-related macular degeneration: the Veterans LAST study (Lutein Antioxidant Supplementation Trial),” Optometry, vol. 75, no. 4, pp. 216–230, 2004. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  33. S. P. Richer, W. Stiles, K. Graham-Hoffman et al., “Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of zeaxanthin and visual function in patients with atrophic age-related macular degeneration: the zeaxanthin and visual function study (ZVF) FDA IND #78, 973,” Optometry, vol. 82, no. 11, pp. 667.e6–680.e6, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  34. J. M. Stringham and B. R. Hammond Jr., “The glare hypothesis of macular pigment function,” Optometry and Vision Science, vol. 84, no. 9, pp. 859–864, 2007. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  35. J. M. Stringham and B. R. Hammond, “Macular pigment and visual performance under glare conditions,” Optometry & Vision Science, vol. 85, no. 2, pp. 82–88, 2008. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar
  36. J. M. Stringham, P. V. Garcia, P. A. Smith, L. N. McLin, and B. K. Foutch, “Macular pigment and visual performance in glare: benefits for photostress recovery, disability glare, and visual discomfort,” Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, vol. 52, no. 10, pp. 7406–7415, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  37. Y. Yao, Q.-H. Qiu, X.-W. Wu, Z.-Y. Cai, S. Xu, and X.-Q. Liang, “Lutein supplementation improves visual performance in Chinese drivers: 1-year randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study,” Nutrition, vol. 29, no. 7-8, pp. 958–964, 2013. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  38. L. J. van Rijn, C. Nischler, D. Gamer et al., “Measurement of stray light and glare: comparison of Nyktotest, Mesotest, stray light meter, and computer implemented stray light meter,” British Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 89, no. 3, pp. 345–351, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  39. J. S. Werner, “Visual problems of the retina during ageing: compensation mechanisms and colour constancy across the life span,” Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 621–645, 1996. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  40. J. S. Werner, “Night vision in the elderly: consequences for seeing through a ‘blue filtering’ intraocular lens,” British Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 89, no. 11, pp. 1518–1521, 2005. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  41. M. A. Mainster and P. L. Turner, “Blue-blocking IOLs vs. short-wavelength visible light: hypothesis-based vs. evidence-based medical practice,” Ophthalmology, vol. 118, no. 1, pp. 1–2, 2011. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  42. M. A. Mainster and P. L. Turner, “Glare's causes, consequences, and clinical challenges after a century of ophthalmic study,” American Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 153, no. 4, pp. 587–593, 2012. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  43. S. Mitra, “Investigating impact of sun glare on transportation safety,” in Proceedings of the Transportation Research Board 87th Annual Meeting, Washington, DC, USA, January 2008.
  44. J. D. Mollon and B. C. Regan, “Spectral distribution of primate cones and of the macular pigment: Matched to properties of the world?” Journal of Optical Technology, vol. 66, no. 10, pp. 847–852, 1999. View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus
  45. D. J. Calkins, “Seeing with S cones,” Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 255–287, 2001. View at Publisher · View at Google Scholar · View at Scopus