Abstract

Sensory gating is a neurophysiological measure of inhibition that is characterized by a reduction in the P50, N100, and P200 event-related potentials to a repeated identical stimulus. It was proposed that abnormal sensory gating is involved in the neural pathological basis of some severe mental disorders. Since then, the prevailing application of sensory gating measures has been in the study of neuropathology associated with schizophrenia and so on. However, sensory gating is not only trait-like but can be also state-like, and measures of sensory gating seemed to be affected by several factors in healthy subjects. The objective of this work was to clarify the roles of acute stress and gender in sensory gating. Data showed acute stress impaired inhibition of P50 to the second click in the paired-click paradigm without effects on sensory registration leading to worse P50 sensory gating and disrupted attention allocation reflected by attenuated P200 responses than control condition, without gender effects. As for N100 and P200 gating, women showed slightly better than men without effects of acute stress. Data also showed slightly larger N100 amplitudes across clicks and significant larger P200 amplitude to the first click for women, suggesting that women might be more alert than men.

1. Introduction

Acute stress is a stereotypical and multimodal response to a present or imminent challenge overcharging an organism [1] and causes the release of many stress hormones and neuromodulators (e.g., [2]), which can change cellular properties of large-scale neuronal populations throughout the brain [3]. The common stress induction paradigms in laboratory include cold water pressing (also called cold pressor stress (CPS); e.g., [4]) and the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST; [5]). Sensory gating is the ability of the central nervous system to filter incoming stimuli and protect a person from being flooded with irrelevant information (e.g., [6]). It is typically measured by using a paired-click paradigm. In such a paradigm, the event-related potential P50 is measured during the presentation of two identical clicks with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 500 ms and a typical interpair interval (IPI) no less than 8 s [7]. The attenuation of the P50 amplitude to the second click (), relative to that of the first click (), is the operational definition of gating inhibition (i.e., gating or sensory gating; [8, 9]). The difference and the ratio between these P50 responses are the means to quantitatively assess gating mechanisms (e.g., [911]).

Freedman and colleagues initially demonstrated that schizophrenic patients and their family members fail to exhibit typical P50 response inhibition to during a paired-click paradigm (e.g., [8, 12]). Since then, sensory gating has been one of the foci of psychopathological researches on schizophrenia. Because of the highly replicable results across studies on schizophrenia (e.g., [1316]) and the findings that filtering deficits also occur in unaffected family members [17, 18], P50 filtering has been investigated as a candidate potential endophenotype for schizophrenia. Additionally, in psychopathological studies, there also have been concerns on N100 and P200 sensory gating, which were assessed using event-related potential N100 and P200, respectively. However, as for normative data regarding the characteristics of sensory gating, up till now, relatively limited studies using healthy volunteers have targeted gender and acute stress, respectively, and have reported inconsistent outcomes (see Table 1).

According to previous studies focusing on gender difference, three studies supported less P50 sensory gating for women [10, 19, 23], and six of the limited documents support no gender difference in P50 sensory gating. The similarly inconsistent findings on the P50 amplitudes for or were reported between genders. As for the influences of acute stress, although adverse P50 sensory gating effects were reported in almost all studies (except for Woods et al. [11]), there were inconsistent findings on the P50 response to the two clicks after acute stress intervention. As for auditory evoked potential components other than P50, as shown in Table 1, the results of N100 and P200 seemed even more inconsistent.

To date, except for a single study, no attempt has been made to investigate the gender difference in the effects of acute stress on sensory gating. During an oral mental arithmetic stressor task, sensory gating ratios were measured to the paired-click paradigm, and women showed disrupted P50 and N100 gating whereas men exhibited only disrupted N100 gating [12]. Although White et al. reported valuable information, several limitations have to be noted. The first is the protocol used to induce acute stress. According to White et al. [12], P50 data were recorded while mental arithmetic was performing, in which the experimental effect was inevitably contaminated by the concurrent working memory task; however, P50 sensory gating was not a complete automatic and preattentive process; it involved top-down modulations and could be influenced by attention manipulation [16, 31, 32]. Furthermore, the muscle artifacts of oral mental arithmetic cannot be ignored. The second is the different interpair intervals (10-14 s for some subjects, while 7-10 s for others), which may deeply affect the P50 response (e.g., [7]), because the sensory gating data were derived from two different studies [27, 28]. The third is numbers of epochs averaged (80 trials for data from White and Yee [27], 60 trials from Yee and White [28]), and the fourth is limited sample size (13 women and 16 men), in addition to some differences in experimental procedures.

Thus, the main objective of this study was to further investigate possible gender differences in the sensory gating after exposure to stressful treatment or control condition. It has been reported that the fluctuation of sex steroid hormone during menstrual cycles affects the performance of working memory [33], proponent response inhibition [34], arousal state [35], and fear conditioning and inhibition [36] in healthy women. Thus, only women during their midluteal phase were included to control the potential effects of menstrual cycle. A body of literature suggests the major neural sources of P50 suppression involves the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (e.g., [9, 37, 38]), where stress hormone receptors are abundant and stress exerts effects on cognitive processes (e.g., [3, 39, 40]). Therefore, sensory gating under acute stress would therefore be expected to be impaired. Additionally, it has been supposed that two processes contribute to the gating deficit, i.e., a reduced sensory registration ( amplitude reduction) and a reduced ability to inhibit the repeated auditory stimulation (lack of reduction of amplitude; e.g., [4143]). Sensory registration and suppression may be differentially affected by acute stress and may show gender difference meanwhile. Thus, another aim of this study is to determine which one plays a big role in the gender and acute stress effects. Given that fewer studies using paired-click paradigm focused on N100 and P200 gating, an exploratory investigation was also made in the current study.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Forty-three healthy university students (25 males, 18 females) were included in the study. One female subject dropped out, data of a male subject was incomplete because of technical failure, and another female was excluded because of failure to obtain reliable P50 response in CPS experimental session. Thus, data were from 24 men (ranged in age from 20 to 23 years, , ) and 16 women (for N100 and P200 data; ranged from 19 to 23; , ). Statistical results of 16 women other than P50 data would not be present in the paper because there was no significant difference from 17-woman sample that were included in the analyses. Only women during their midluteal phase (with regular menstrual cycles, days 16 to 24) were included to control possible gender and ovarian cycle effects on adrenocortical reactivity [44, 45]. Participants were asked to refrain from caffeine, alcohol, tea, and smoking within 6 h before the experimental sessions. The volunteers were recruited by announcements and received financial compensation. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Minnan Normal University. All participants were naive to the purpose of the study and gave their written informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.

2.2. Procedure

After a participant’s arrival, he or she was allowed to rest briefly, and then, preexperimental saliva sample (to measure cortisol level) was taken, and systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and heart rate (HR) were recorded at the same time to evaluate participant’s physical baseline. Then, participants filled out the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; [46]), Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II; [47]), and Anxiety Inventory [48]. Data of Trait Anxiety Inventory was collected during their first experimental session. Then, participants were exposed to either the CPS treatment or the warm water (control) treatment (adapted from) [4]. Immediately after treatment, all subjects had a rest, and then SBP, DBP, and HR were measured at about 4 min after CPS or control procedures. Then, subjects were engaged in the experimental task, and meanwhile, EEG data was collected. Further, saliva samples were taken immediately after the task (about 15 min after treatment). The method of salivary cortisol measurement was described in Yang et al. [49]. All salivary samples were stored at -40°C, and analyses were completed within about one month.

This experiment was conducted by adopting a within-subjects design, in which CPS and control procedures were, respectively, applied by an interval of at least 24 h, and treatment order was counterbalanced. Subjects were instructed to submerge their hands and wrists in cold water (6° to 9°C) for 5 min for CPS session while in warm water (35° to 38°C) during control session (adapted from [4]). To avoid any influence of the circadian profiles of adrenocortical reactivity and cognitive ability, CPS or control procedures were conducted in the same time period of the experiment day and the other experimental procedures were the very same.

2.3. Stimuli and EEG Recording and Analysis
2.3.1. Auditory Stimulation

About 5 min after treatment, paired clicks of 2000 Hz, 95 dB SPL tones, and 4 ms in duration were delivered via headphones with 50 dB SPL background white noise. The sound intensity was measured at the subject’s ear by a sound meter. All 60 paired clicks were separated by a 500 ms interval, and interpair interval was random ranged from 7.5 to 10 s in order to allow brain activity to return to baseline (e.g., [6, 7]).

2.3.2. EEG Recording

Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in an electromagnetically shielded room, wearing headphones, and instructed to sit comfortably and still, close their eyes, relax, and listen to clicks. All subjects were monitored for signs of drowsiness by visual observation and EEG monitoring because P50 component is sleep-state dependent (e.g., [11]). Brain electrical activity was recorded at Fz, Cz, Pz, F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, and O2 sites using Ag/AgCl electrodes mounted on an elastic cap (Brain Product, München, Germany), with references on FCz, and a ground electrode on the medial frontal aspect. Vertical electrooculograms (EOGs) were recorded supra- and infraorbitally at the left eye. The horizontal EOG was recorded from the left versus right orbital rim. The EEG and EOG were amplified using a 0.05 to 100 Hz bandpass and were continuously digitized at 1000 Hz/channel during online recording. All interelectrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ.

2.3.3. EEG Analysis

Offline, the data were referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids, digitally filtered at 10–50 Hz for P50 and 1–30 Hz for N100 and P200, a 50 Hz notch filter and both a roll-off of 24 dB/octave, segmented (–100 to 200 ms for P50; –100 to 400 ms for N100 and P200), and baseline-corrected (100 ms). Trials containing artifacts (activity in any channel exceeded 75 μV) were removed from further analyses. Totally, 75–100% of the epochs (45–60 trials) were included in the N100 and P200 analyses and 77-100% (46-60 trials) for P50. There is no statistical difference in number of epochs of each condition.

After averaging, according to the procedures of former studies (e.g., [11, 12, 16, 27, 38]), latencies and amplitudes of the P30, N40, P50, N100, and P200 at Cz were analyzed on the basis of automatic peak detection in combination with a visual inspection according to the waveforms drawn using Excel 2007. The P50 component was defined as the most positive response between 45 and 90 ms poststimulus preceded by a P30 wave in a 15–45 ms range. The N100 and P200 component was defined as a prominent negative-positive complex (N100: 80–180 ms, P200: 120–250 ms). P50 amplitude was normally measured relative to the N40 (defined as the most negative response between P30 and P50 latencies; if no identifiable P30, then between 30 ms and P50 latency). If no identifiable N40 happened under any condition, all P50 amplitudes of this subject were measured relative to the prestimulus baseline, and this solution was also used in case of negative P50 gating ratios. N100 and P200 amplitudes were measured relative to the prestimulus baseline. As for components of , they were additionally determined by reference to the component latencies (i.e., ±15 ms away from latency of P50 for P50, ±30 ms for N100 and ±35 ms for P200). When no amplitude was identifiable for , the subject’s response was excluded from further analysis (one case for P50). If this was the case for , it was interpreted as maximum suppression and the amplitude was set to zero in accordance [50]. Gating indices were calculated as gating ratio () as well as gating difference ().

3. Results

3.1. Mood, Trait Anxiety, and Physiological Measurements

Results of trait anxiety test demonstrated no difference between males (, ) and females (, , ). To evaluate potential differences in baseline mood variables (positive and negative affect, state anxiety, and depression), mixed measure ANOVAs were conducted with treatment (CPS vs. control) as a within-subjects factor and gender as a between-subjects factor. The ANOVA showed no main effects of gender (, 0.29, 0.48, and 0.59, respectively), treatment (, 0.64, 0.24, and 0.77, respectively), and no interactions (, 0.22, 0.22, and 0.88, respectively) (Table 2). Mixed measure ANOVAs were also conducted on cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity with two within-subjects factors: treatment (CPS vs. control) and timing (baseline and after treatment) and gender as another factor to evaluate the effect of experimental manipulation. The results showed significant main effects of treatment in HR [, , ], DBP [, , ], SBP [, , ], and cortisol concentrations [, , ] and significant main effects of gender. Specifically, data showed significantly higher HR () and cortisol concentrations () for females, while higher DBP () and SBP () for males. The results also showed significant interactions of to DBP [, , ], SBP [, , ], and cortisol [, , ]. Further analysis showed no difference of blood pressures between baseline and after-treatment data during control session while in CPS session there were significantly higher after-treatment DBP (), SBP (), and cortisol concentrations (), relative to baseline data.

3.2. P50 Latencies, Amplitudes, and Sensory Gating

The grand averaged auditory evoked potentials for both genders, during two experimental sessions, are presented in Figure 1, and descriptive results on P50 measures can be found in Table 3. P50 amplitudes and latencies to peak were evaluated by performing separate 2 (gender) × 2 (stimuli: vs. ) × 2 (treatment: CPS vs. control) mixed measure ANOVAs. The ANOVA showed the P50 latencies did not show any main effects and interactions (all ). However, the P50 amplitudes showed a significant main effect of stimuli [, , ] and a significant interaction of [, , ], but no main effect of gender or other interactions. Further analysis showed P50 amplitude to during CPS experimental session was significantly larger than that to during control session (), while there is no difference for P50 amplitude to between two sessions. Additionally, P50 amplitude to was significantly larger than that to for both sessions ( for CPS and for control).

P50 gating ratios and gating difference were evaluated by performing 2 (gender) × 2 (treatment) mixed measure ANOVAs. The results showed no gender main effect () and interaction (), but a significant main effect of treatment [, , ] for gating ratios. As for gating difference, the same results were obtained, which showed a better gating function after control procedures than that of CPS [, , ], without gender effect () and interaction ().

3.3. N100 Latencies, Amplitudes, and Sensory Gating

The grand averaged potentials are presented in Figure 2, and descriptive results on N100 are shown in Table 3. N100 amplitudes and latencies were evaluated by performing separate 2 (gender) ×2 (stimuli: vs. ) × 2 (treatment: CPS vs. control) mixed measure ANOVAs.

According to the ANOVA, there was a significant main effect of stimuli with longer latencies for than [, , ] and a borderline main effect of gender [, , ], with slightly longer latencies for males than females, but there was no main effect of treatment and no interactions on latency (all ). The results also showed a significant main effect of stimuli [, , , ] and of gender [, , , larger for females than males] for the mean N100 amplitudes and a borderline significant interaction of [, , ] without treatment effect. Further analysis showed relatively larger N100 amplitudes in women than men for () and ().

N100 gating ratios and gating difference were evaluated by performing 2 (gender) × 2 (treatment) mixed measure ANOVAs. The results showed no main effects of gender () and treatment () and no interaction () for gating ratios. The data showed similar results for gating difference, but a borderline main effect of gender [, , ], with slightly better N100 gating function for females.

3.4. P200 Latencies, Amplitudes, and Sensory Gating

The grand averaged potentials are presented in Figure 2, and descriptive results of P200 can be found in Table 3. P200 amplitudes and latencies were evaluated by performing 2 (gender) × 2 (stimuli) × 2 (treatment) mixed measure ANOVAs. As for latencies, there was only a significant main effect of stimuli [, , ], with longer P200 latencies for than . As for P200 amplitudes, the results showed there were significant main effects of treatment [, , ], stimuli [, , ], and gender [, , ] and a significant interaction of [, , ]. Further analysis showed P200 responses were attenuated under CPS than control condition (), and females had significantly larger P200 amplitude than males (), but there was no notable difference in response amplitude between genders ().

P200 gating ratios and gating difference were evaluated by performing 2 (gender) × 2 (treatment) mixed measure ANOVAs. The results showed no main effects of gender () and treatment () and no interaction () for gating ratios. The data showed similar results for gating differences, but a significant gender effect [, , ], with better P200 gating function for females.

4. Discussion

To characterize the response to the CPS, salivary cortisol and cardiovascular response were assessed. The results revealed significantly higher blood pressures, as well as an increased activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis (reflected by higher cortisol concentrations) after CPS treatment, compared to the control procedure session. The findings are well in line with the previous studies (e.g., [4, 51]) and indicate the successful induction of a neuroendocrine stress response [1]. Moreover, a comparable basis was demonstrated by the baseline psychological measures across all conditions.

In the paired-click paradigm, sensory gating is operationally defined as the ratio of the amplitude of the response to divided by that of or the amplitude difference between the two clicks (e.g., [911]). Higher ratios and smaller absolute difference values mean worse sensory gating, which could be a result of lower amplitudes in response to , thus weaker registration functions, or higher amplitudes in response to , thus weaker inhibition with repetition, according to some authors (e.g., [4143]). In this view, this study showed diminished P50 gating due to enhanced amplitudes to significantly under CPS relative to control condition, while P50 amplitudes to remaining unaffected, which indicated that CPS disrupted subjects’ capacity of inhibition with repetition, but not sensory registration. These results were in right accordance with Johnson and Adler [26] and Atchley [30]. In addition, Ermutlu et al. [29] used an oddball paradigm and also found CPS impaired P50 sensory gating. As for Woods et al. [11] reporting no evidence on impairment in P50 gating, the most likely explanation is that the CPS procedure used in that study was so weak as only 50 seconds of immersing in cold water that the stress stimuli may not be strong enough to trigger the abundant releasing of norepinephrine (NE) and cortisol to disrupt the sensory gating. A much stronger CPS procedure was used in the current study, and both data of blood pressures and saliva samples proved successful induction of a neuroendocrine stress response. As was known, acute stress can lead to the release of NE from widely distributed synapses, including abundant projections to the PFC, and the rapid activation of the prefrontal dopamine system, and on a slightly longer time scale, the release of cortisol, whose receptors are also abundant in the PFC [3, 39, 40, 52]. However, it was proposed that PFC should be the major neural source of P50 suppression by many documents (e.g., [9, 38, 5356]). Thus, it is reasonable that acute stress disrupts P50 sensory gating via the adverse effects of stress hormones and neuromodulators on PFC which might result in impairments of the inhibition of redundant information.

However, White and Yee [27] reported a reduction of P50 amplitude during mental arithmetic stressor, which are different from the results of Johnson and Adler [26], Atchley [30] and the current study that acute stress resulted in an increase of P50 amplitude with unchanged P50 amplitude. Given that consciously directing attention toward the clicks enhanced P50 amplitude in healthy subjects (e.g., [16, 31]), oral mental arithmetic stressor task during passively listening task makes less attention resources switched to the clicks and might then a reduction of P50 amplitude, while in the cases of Johnson and Adler [26], Atchley [30] and the current study, CPS was used to induce stress state and there was no specific cognitive processes involved.

No gender difference in P50 amplitudes and sensory gating was reported in the current study, which was in accordance with previous studies [6, 2221, 24, 25]. Freedman, Adler, and Waldo [20] also reported no gender difference in P50 sensory gating, but they found women had higher P50 amplitude to than men. However, Franks et al. [19] and Hetrick et al. [10] reported women had significantly larger P50 responses to , but Fuerst et al. [23] reported men scored higher than women in P50 amplitude to . It is not likely to interpret the discrepancy properly by gender difference in the neuroanatomical origins of the auditory P50 response and inhibition, because it was documented that there was no known gender difference in brain structures or neuronal systems relevant to P50, such as auditory cortex, thalamus, and PFC ([57]; e.g., [9, 10]), and the discrepancy is mostly state dependent. As mentioned earlier, menstrual cycles could affect several cognitive processing. In particular, Goldstein et al. [35, 58] found menstrual cycle modulated women’s arousal state with less cortical control over the amygdala during early follicular due to low level of estrogen, but an attenuation of brain activity during midcycle in the presence of higher levels of estrogen. Therefore, given different arousals leading to different baseline P50 responses [11, 59] and the interplay between acute stress and sex steroid hormone (for a review, see [60]), the mechanisms under the discrepancy is more likely estrogen level dependent.

The results of the current study showed no gender difference in the CPS effects on P50 amplitudes and sensory gating. However, White et al. [12] reported a significant gender difference with a significantly reduced P50 to but not during mental arithmetic stressor for women, not for men, but they did not report any information about menstrual cycles. Therefore, in addition to the defects in design mentioned earlier, the most likely explanation would be that more early follicular phase women participated in that study, while female subjects used in the current study were all in their midluteal phase (days 16 to 24) during which there was a steady rise in estrogen levels forming a second estrogen peak in menstrual cycles [61] and then women might be more comparable to men in arousal and sensory gating because of more cortical control over the amygdala due to high level of estrogen. Future studies should select healthy female subjects during their different menstrual cycles based on direct measurements of gonadal hormone levels instead of self-reported data to precisely determine menstrual phases to further confirm and extend the present findings.

As reviewed earlier, relatively fewer studies using paired-click procedure focused on N100 and P200 sensory gating. And the relevant findings were also inconsistent. According to Hetrick et al. [10], women had significantly higher N100 amplitudes to and worse N100 sensory gating compared to men, but no significant differences were found at P200, and Fuerst et al. [23] found that men had larger N100 and P200 amplitude to and better N100 and P200 sensory gating relative to women. However, other studies reported no gender difference [22, 24]. Data of the current study showed women had larger N100 and P200 amplitudes to and better N100 and P200 gating function. In addition to the possible effects of arousal due to fluctuations of gonadal hormones mentioned earlier, potential manipulation of attention because of nonstandardized instructions across studies might be an alternative interpretation. Gjini et al. [31] reported attention status had a significant effect on N100 and P200 amplitudes and gating. However, there is no specific control for attention factors, according to typical instructions in paired-click paradigms. For example, subjects are usually requested to remain awake and try to decrease their eye movements, close (e.g., [10] and the current study) or open eyes, fixate on an object (e.g., [22]) or not, listen passively or intently to clicks (e.g., [22, 24]), keep relaxed or not, and so on. Thus, different baseline attention status due to different emphases in instructions, combined and interacted with fluctuations of gonadal hormones, leads to complex results.

Some studies suggested that in paired-click paradigm N100 could reflect initial direction of attention, and P200 reflects early allocation of attention, initial conscious experience, and entrance into working memory of the stimulus [31, 62, 63]. Therefore, women in the current study might be more alert, reflected by larger N100 amplitudes than men both to and . And in the end, women allocated more attention resources to relevant information than men, reflected by significantly larger P200 amplitude for females than males. Additionally, the current study also indicated that CPS disrupted allocation of attention resources, reflected by attenuated activation during P200 time window for both clicks after CPS than control procedures, which was in accordance with the majority of the findings in attention studies, and should involve the mechanism that acute stress impairs normal selective attention via disruptions of higher PFC functions (as a review, see [64]).

As for the effects of acute stress on sensory gating of N100 and P200, the current study found no stress effect, which was in accordance with Ermutlu et al. [29]. However, White et al. [12] using mental arithmetic stressor reported disrupted N100 gating ratios. In typical paired-click paradigm, only pairs of identical clicks are presented and cognitive processing resources are always sufficient. This might be a possible explanation for no effect of CPS on N100 amplitude and gating indices in Ermutlu et al. [29] and this current study, but significant stress effects on N100 response and gating ratio in White et al. [12] because of the potential inadequate process resources allocated in paired-click listening task due to concurrent mental arithmetic processing and the impairment of inhibition function due to stressful situation.

At last, the data showed significantly longer latencies to than both for N100 and P200 and a similar trend for P50 although not statistically significant in this current study, which were in accordance with former studies [10, 23, 24, 65]. We cannot interpret the results properly due to scarcity of relevant documents. Probably, peak latencies reflect the depth of information processing and cognitive process to is incomplete because of sensory gating, and as a result, the latency to is longer than . The data of this study also showed a trend of longer N100 latencies for men than women, and it might be the fact that the depth of cognitive process during N100 time window was less for women than men, although as mentioned earlier, women might be more alert and allocate more attention resources to relevant information than men, according to larger N100 and P200 amplitudes than men both to and . In a word, peak latencies in the paired-click paradigm need more concerns and relevant data need to be better understood in the future, but at least the current latency data are in accordance with former studies, which assures in some extent that the components of P50, N100, and P200 are extracted precisely.

As for the application of paired-click paradigm in neural pathological studies, based on this study, more emphasis should be paid on the control of all kinds of factors that might induce subjects a stressful state, and we suggest the importance of monitoring stress and anxiety levels of subjects, and furthermore, N100 and P200 gating indices should be cautiously used because of their strong correlations with high-order cognitive process. But no matter how does one think of the applications of the P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating in pathological fields, the primary focus of the work is to standardize the recording of gating data and the extracting procedure of the components.

Data Availability

All data included in this study are available upon request by contact with the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that there are no competing financial interests.

Authors’ Contributions

ZX, HL, and YL designed the study; ZX, SG, and WW did the experiments and analyzed the data. ZX, YL, and SG wrote the paper. Zengyou Xin and Simeng Gu contributed equally to this paper.

Acknowledgments

This study is supported by the following: Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science-Shenzhen Fundamental Research Institutions (2019SHIBS0003); National Natural Science Foundation of China (31871130 and 31671150); Guangdong Key Project (2018B030335001); Guangdong Major Program (2016KZDXM009); Innovative Team Program in Higher Education of Guangdong (2015KCXTD009); Fujian Provincial Department of Education Projects (JA15320); Shenzhen Basic Research Scheme (JCYJ20150729104249783); and Shenzhen Peacock Plan (KQTD2015033016104926).