Journal of Obesity

Journal of Obesity / 2012 / Article
Special Issue

Self-Regulation, Motivation, and Psychosocial Factors in Weight Management

View this Special Issue

Research Article | Open Access

Volume 2012 |Article ID 846401 | 10 pages | https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/846401

Eating and Psychological Profiles of Women with Higher Depressive Symptoms Who Are Trying to Lose Weight

Academic Editor: Jutta Mata
Received05 Dec 2011
Revised23 Apr 2012
Accepted15 Jun 2012
Published07 Aug 2012

Abstract

The aim of this study was to determine whether women with higher depressive symptoms differed from women with lower depressive symptoms on early weight-loss, eating behaviors and psychological profiles. Among a sample of 45 overweight/obese women who had undertaken a self-initiated weight-loss attempt, two groups were formed based on scores from the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II), one with lower depressive symptoms (BDI-II < 10; ๐‘› = 2 1 ) and one with higher depressive symptoms (BDI โ‰ฅ 10; ๐‘› = 2 4 ). Even if some women in the higher depressive symptom group did not reach the clinical cut-off for depression (BDI = 14), this group tended to lose less weight in the first two months of their weight-loss attempt and to show a more disturbed eating and psychological profile compared to the group with lower depressive symptoms. In addition, among women with higher depressive symptoms, eating and psychological variables were systematically related to one another whereas these variables were not related among the other group. Results highlight the relevance of considering the presence of depressive symptoms as a marker of clinical severity among the overweight/obese population, and suggest that the BDI-II could be an interesting screening instrument to identify this particular subgroup.

1. Introduction

Obesity jeopardizes public health worldwide [1]. Despite the numerous weight-loss programs available, prevalence rates of obesity and overweight relentlessly increase, which suggests that, as many as they are, current weight-loss programs do not succeed in reducing, or even stabilizing, the proportion of individuals who are overweight or obese. One potential explanation might be that most individuals are not able to achieve or maintain weight-loss recommendations in the long term. Individual characteristics could account for these difficulties to regulate body weight. In fact, overweight/obese individuals seem to form a particularly heterogeneous population divided into specific subgroups that can be characterized by distinctive psychological features [2โ€“4].

In particular, women suffer more from their weight problems than men and report more weight preoccupation and body dissatisfaction, regardless of their age, ethnic group, education level, and body mass index (BMI) [3, 5โ€“7]. Women are also overrepresented in weight-loss programs, where 85 to 90% of participants are women [8โ€“11]. In weight-loss studies, obese women also show greater distress than men; for example, they generally show more depressive symptoms and are more likely to report repetitive and unsuccessful dieting in the past [12, 13]. These considerations suggest that women represent an at-risk subgroup of obese individuals.

Another subgroup of obese individuals that may present distinctive features leading to strenuous weight regulation is the one characterized by a higher level of depressive symptoms. Overweight/obese women seem to be more vulnerable to depressive symptoms when compared to overweight/obese men and to normal-weight women [14]. Yet, all obese women are not systematically characterized by depressive symptoms, and the correlation between obesity and depression is significant but weak. In fact, while some studies reported a positive association [15โ€“20] as well as a longitudinal bidirectional relationship between these two concepts [21], other studies reported a weak or no association at all [3, 22, 23]. These findings may suggest that, while some overweight/obese women are asymptomatic, another subgroup of women does show depressive symptoms, which may interact with other factors affecting eating behaviors and weight-loss efforts. Recently, several research groups have observed the eating and the psychological differences between these two groups.

Werrij and her colleagues [24] studied this phenomenon among a sample of 166 women seeking treatment at a weight-loss center. Half of the sample was categorized as the higher depressive symptoms group, whereas the other half was identified as the lower depressive symptoms group on the basis of the Beck Depression Inventory score (median = 10). Women with higher depressive symptoms had higher weight, shape, and eating concerns as well as lower self-esteem. They also reported higher dietary restraint and greater binge eating. However, in this sample, women with higher depressive symptoms presented a higher BMI than the other women, which could account for the observed differences.

Subsequent studies have corroborated these results. Cluster analyses led to the identification of a higher negative affect group and a lower negative affect group among a sample of obese/overweight women from the community [25, 26]. Women with higher negative affect were younger and reported higher weight, shape and eating concerns, more binge eating, and lower self-esteem. Moreover, when exposed to negative mood induction, only women in the higher negative affect group presented disinhibited eating. These differences between the two groups could not be attributed to body weight, since groups had similar BMI.

Another study conducted among a sample of 156 overweight/obese weight-preoccupied women, involved in a Health-at-Every-Size program [27] replicated previous findings, and went further by showing that women with higher depressive symptoms presented some particular personality traits, notably high neuroticism and low agreeableness as well as problematic coping strategies, such as more emotion-oriented and avoidance strategies, and less task-oriented strategies. Again, these differences could not be attributed to body weight since there was no difference in BMI between groups. However, in this study, numerous exclusion criteria were used to identify eligible participants (e.g., no eating disorders, stable weight for two months, no clinical depression, etc.), which may limit the representativeness of the sample. Thus, taken altogether, these results suggest that women who present higher depressive symptoms may exhibit distinctive features that could impede weight-loss efforts.

Other studies support the hypothesis that depressive symptoms could be related to less successful weight-loss outcomes [13, 28โ€“31]. Indeed, there is a significant, though weak, negative correlation between weight-loss and depressive symptoms ( ๐‘Ÿ = โˆ’ 0 . 1 5 ; ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 1 ) [13]. In the same order of idea, it has been shown that women with higher depressive symptoms are less likely to complete a weight-loss program or to succeed in losing weight [29โ€“51]. Fabricatore and his colleagues [29] suggested that demands which are inherent to weight-loss programs may be too challenging for women with significant depressive symptoms.

Up until now, no study has dichotomized women based on depressive symptoms to observe their weight-loss outcomes. Moreover, most weight-loss studies have been conducted using well-controlled, randomized protocols, and structured weight-loss programs. Yet, some authors suggest that outcomes from controlled studies using structured weight-loss programs may differ markedly from what happens to women who try to lose weight on their own. In fact, these women are likely to be less rigorous and to lose smaller amount of body weight [13, 32, 33].

The study aimed at deepening the understanding of subgroups of overweight/obese women separated on the basis of depressive symptoms. Specifically, the objective of this study was to determine, in a naturalistic study, whether women with higher depressive symptoms differed from women with lower depressive symptoms on early weight-loss outcomes as well as on eating and psychological profiles, and to examine the links between all these variables among the different groups. We expected that higher depressive symptoms women would lose less weight compared to women with lower depressive symptoms and that they would present more problematic eating behaviors and less positive psychological features.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Forty-five women who corresponded to inclusion criteria (being premenopausal, i.e., reporting a regular menstrual cycle for the last three months, having a BMI of 25โ€‰kg/m2 or more, and having initiated a weight-loss attempt in the last week) completed baseline measures (questionnaires and physical measurements) (week 1). Thirty-six women came to the subsequent weighing visits (week 2 to week 8). Among women who dropped out of the study ( ๐‘› = 9 ), four moved away, two decided to stop for personal reasons and three could not be reached by the research team during the course of the study.

Women were aged between 21 and 56 years ( ๐‘€ a g e = 3 7 . 6 ยฑ 9 . 6 ). Twenty-four (53.3%) were in a relationship (married or in couple) while 21 (46.6%) were not (i.e., single, separated, or divorced). All women completed a high-school diploma, and 26 (57.3%) completed a university degree. At baseline, mean BMI was 3 0 . 5 8 ยฑ 4 . 8 3 ( ๐‘€ B M I for the lower depressive symptoms group = 3 1 . 0 5 ยฑ 4 . 7 9 ; ๐‘€ B M I for the higher depressive symptoms group = 3 0 . 1 6 ยฑ 4 . 9 3 ). All women had initiated a weight-loss attempt one week or less before the baseline visit. Weight-loss methods ranged from reducing portions or trying to exercise more by themselves ( ๐‘› = 2 9 ), enrolling in commercial weight-loss programs with strict eating rules such as Weight Watchers and Minรงavi programs ( ๐‘› = 1 3 ), or reading self-help books ( ๐‘› = 3 ). There was no significant difference between groups concerning the methods that were used to lose weight (Pearson chi-squares were not significant, ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 6 8 8 to 0.905). Only six women (13.3%) had never tried to lose weight before. Women had become preoccupied with their weight around the end of their adolescence or at the beginning of adulthood ( ๐‘€ a g e o f ๏ฌ r s t p r e o c c u p a t i o n s = 1 7 . 6 9 ยฑ 7 . 7 1 years).

2.2. Procedure

For recruitment, advertisements were placed in commercial weight-loss and fitness centers, as well as in different places on the Laval University campus. In addition, emails were sent to all students and staff of the university campus.

All participants were met at baseline (week 1), were told about the aims and procedures of the study, and were asked to read and sign the consent form approved by the ethics committee of the university. Subsequently, detailed questions regarding their current weight-loss attempt were asked. Participants were then measured and weighed. They were asked to complete self-reported questionnaires at baseline only. Participants came back once a week for the first six weeks (week 2 to 6), and once, two weeks later (week 8), to be weighed. Weekly weighing for the first six weeks was proposed in order to carefully monitor the early weight-loss process as well as to improve womenโ€™s motivation to participate in the study.

2.3. Measures
2.3.1. Physical Measurements

Height and weight were measured and BMI was calculated based on standard procedures [34].

2.3.2. Sociodemographic and Weight-Loss-Related Information

Sociodemographic information such as age, education, and marital status was collected. Age at first weight concerns and first weight-loss attempt, number of previous weight-loss attempts, and description of current weight-loss method were included in the sociodemographic questionnaire.

2.3.3. Depressive Symptoms

The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II [35]), a 21-item questionnaire, was used to measure depressive symptoms. The BDI-II presents a good internal consistency ( ๐›ผ = 0 . 9 2 in a nonpsychiatric population) and a good test-retest reliability (one week: ๐‘Ÿ = 0 . 9 3 ) [35]. Severity of depressive symptoms increases with scores (0โ€“13 = minimal symptoms; 14โ€“19 = mild depression; 20โ€“28 = moderate depression; 29 or more = severe depression).

2.3.4. Eating Behaviors

The Three-Factor-Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ [36]), a well-known 51-item questionnaire divided in three subscales (Cognitive dietary restraint, Disinhibition, and Susceptibility to hunger), was used to measure eating behaviors. The internal consistency is good for the three factors of the questionnaire ( ๐‘Ž = 0 . 9 2 , 0.91, and 0.85) [36].

2.3.5. Motivation

The Global Motivation Scale (GMS [37]), a 24-item questionnaire, was used to evaluate six different types of motivation (intrinsic, integrated, identified, introjected, external motivation, and amotivation), based on the Self-Determination Theory of Deci and Ryan [38]. Reliability and validity of the GMS have been established by five independent studies [37]. The Regulation of Eating Behaviors Scale (REBS [39]) was used to evaluate motivation to regulate eating behaviors specifically. The REBS internal consistency is good ( ๐‘Ž between 0.72 and 0.95 depending of the subscale [39]).

2.3.6. Quality of Life

The Impact of Weight on Quality of Life Questionnaire (IWQOL-Lite [40]) is a 31-item questionnaire that measures quality of life related to weight. The IWQOL-Lite shows high internal consistency ( ๐›ผ = 0 . 9 6 for total score) and satisfying convergent and discriminant validity [41].

2.3.7. Body Esteem

The Body-Esteem Scale (BES [42]), a 23-item questionnaire used to measure the level of satisfaction of individuals toward their body image, is divided in three subscales: Appearance, Weight, and Attribution. Very high internal consistency coefficients ( ๐›ผ = 0 . 9 2 , 0.81, and 0.94) as well as good temporal stability have been demonstrated for each scale ( ๐›ผ = 0 . 8 9 , 0.92, and 0.83) [42].

2.3.8. Self-Esteem

The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES [43]) was used to measure self-esteem. The 10-item questionnaire measures the global self-concept and the feeling of personal worth. Internal consistency varies from 0.83 [44] to 0.99 [45] and test-retest reliability is 0.82 [46].

2.3.9. Personality

The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI [47]) was used to measure pathological and nonpathological dimensions of personality in terms of Temperament (Harm avoidance, Novelty seeking, Reward Dependence, and Persistence) and Character (Self-Determination, Cooperativeness, and Self-Transcendence). The TCI shows a good internal consistency ( ๐‘Ž > 0 . 8 0 depending of subscales) [48].

2.4. Statistical Analyses

First, descriptive analyses were computed for the whole sample. Means, standard deviations and Pearsonโ€™s correlations between the different variables were computed with the SPSS software (version 13.0). As suggested by Werrij and her colleagues [24], two groups of women were formed based on the BDI score (median = 10); a first group, with a BDI score of less than 10 ( ๐‘› = 2 1 ) and a second group, with a BDI score of 10 or more ( ๐‘› = 2 4 ). To test differences between groups, univariate analyses of variance were computed for the following dependent variables: baseline BMI, number of previous weight loss attempts, percentage of weight loss, and impact of weight on quality of life. Multivariate analyses of variance were computed for age variables (model 1 = current age, age at first weight concerns, age at first weight-loss attempt), eating behaviors (model 2 = cognitive dietary restraint, disinhibition, susceptibility to hunger), body- and self-esteem (model 3 = self-esteem, body esteem related to appearance, weight and attribution), personality (model 4 = novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, persistence, self-determination, cooperativeness, self-transcendence), motivation to regulate eating behaviors (model 5 = intrinsic, integrated, identified, introjected, external motivation and amotivation), and global motivation (model 6 = intrinsic, identified, introjected, external motivation, and amotivation). Bonferroni correction was used to correct for the number of comparisons, ( ๐›ผ = 0 . 0 5 / 1 0 tests = 0.005), meaning that results were considered significant if ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 5 . Effect sizes (Cohenโ€™s ๐‘‘ ) were also calculated; an effect size of 0.3 was considered a small effect, 0.5, a medium effect, and 0.8, a large effect [49]. The number of previous weight-loss attempts was logged 10 transformed to correct for skewness.

3. Results

Means and standard deviations for each group (higher versus lower depressive symptoms) concerning age-, weight-, and eating-related variables, as well as psychological and motivational variables are presented, respectively, in Tables 1, 2, and 3. The mean BDI score for the overall sample was 1 1 . 2 9 ยฑ 1 . 5 8 . For the lower depressive symptoms group, the mean BDI was 3 . 6 2 ยฑ 0 . 5 8 , ranging from 0 to 8, whereas for the higher depressive symptoms group, the mean BDI was 1 8 ยฑ 2 ranging from 10 to 50. In accordance, even though this groupโ€™s mean score was above the clinical cut-off of 14 for mild depression, some women of this group did not reach this clinical cut-off (score between 10โ€“13; ๐‘› = 1 2 ), meaning that the group cannot be labelled as โ€œdepressed.โ€ Among other women belonging to the high depressive symptoms group, four presented mild depression (score between 14 and 19), five reported moderate depression (score between 20 and 28), and three were severely depressed (score over 29).


VariablesLower depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 2 1 )Higher depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 2 4 )Anova MANOVA ๐‘ƒ
๐‘€ (ET) ๐‘€ (ET)

Baseline BMI31.05 (4.79)30.16 (4.93) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 3 7 0.548
Agea41.56 (8.82)35.50 (9.38) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 4 . 0 7 0.050
Age of first weight concernsa20.56 (7.85)14.45 (6.14) ๐น ( 1 , 4 0 ) = 7 . 2 5 0.011
Age of first weight-loss attemptsa24.69 (9.23)17.59 (6.06) ๐น ( 1 , 3 8 ) = 8 . 2 0 0.007
2-month weight lossโ€‰โ€‰โ€‰โ€‰
โ€ƒ(in %)2.74 (2.43)1.13 (1.66) ๐น ( 1 , 3 4 ) = 5 . 4 1 0.026
โ€ƒ(in pounds)5.05 (5.01)1.93 (3.00)
Number of previous weight-loss attempts5.41 (4.77)8.04 (10.43) ๐น ( 1 , 3 8 ) = 0 . 7 9 0.380
TFEQ-Restraintb10.19 (3.23)9.67 (4.87) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 1 8 0.678
TFEQ-Disinhibitionb8.10 (3.49)11.21 (2.75) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 1 . 1 7 0.002*
TFEQ-Hungerb3.52 (3.08)8.58 (3.54) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 5 . 8 4 <0.001*

* ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 5 (Bonferroni correction); aMANOVA 1; bMANOVA 2.

VariablesLower depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 2 1 )Higher depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 2 4 )Anova MANOVAP
๐‘€ (ET) ๐‘€ (ET)

BESโ€”Appearancec1.69 (0.65)1.10 (0.65) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 9 . 2 7 0.004*
BESโ€”Weightc1.21 (0.63)0.69 (0.50) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 9 . 4 4 0.004*
BESโ€”Attributionc1.99 (0.52)1.68 (0.57) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 5 8 0.065
RSESc34.52 (4.38)27.38 (6.86) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 6 . 8 0 <0.001*
IWQL79.38 (10.88)68.34 (16.00) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 7 . 1 1 0.011
TCIโ€”Novelty Seekingd21.14 (4.27)21.29 (7.54) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 0 6 0.937
TCIโ€”Harm Avoidanced13.19 (4.56)16.92 (7.98) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 5 6 0.066
TCIโ€”Reward Dependenced15.19 (3.78)15.21 (4.27) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 0 0 0.988
TCIโ€”Persistenced5.00 (2.02)5.54 (2.15) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 7 5 0.391
TCIโ€”Self-Determinationd35.48 (3.67)23.88 (8.89) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 1 . 0 8 <0.001*
TCIโ€”Cooperativenessd37.33 (2.54)32.04 (5.93) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 4 . 3 8 <0.001*
TCIโ€”Self-Transcendenced11.29 (6.24)12.08 (6.95) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 1 6 0.689

* ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 5 (Bonferroni correction); cMANOVA 3; dMANOVA 4.

VariablesLower depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 21)Higher depressive SX ( ๐‘› = 24)MANOVA ๐‘ƒ
๐‘€ (SD) ๐‘€ (SD)

REBSโ€”Intrinsice5.15 (1.44)4.34 (1.44) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 5 8 0.065
REBSโ€”Integratede5.33 (1.22)4.24 (1.51) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 7 . 0 3 0.011
REBSโ€”Identifiede6.36 (0.73)5.88 (1.09) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 . 9 5 0.093
REBSโ€”Introjectede3.02 (1.03)3.98 (1.71) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 4 . 9 8 0.031
REBSโ€”Externale1.54 (1.02)2.40 (1.86) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 5 4 0.066
REBSโ€”Amotivatione1.17 (0.37)1.65 (0.91) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 5 . 0 8 0.029
GMSโ€”Intrinsicf5.64 (1.15)5.14 (1.19) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 . 0 1 0.164
GMSโ€”Identifiedf5.51 (0.94)5.16 (0.94) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 . 5 0 0.228
GMSโ€”Introjectedf3.43 (1.31)4.10 (1.26) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 . 9 8 0.092
GMSโ€”Externalf3.43 (1.31)4.10 (1.26) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 . 9 8 0.092
GMSโ€”Amotivationf1.59 (0.61)2.20 (1.39) ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 3 6 0.074

* ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 5 (Bonferroni correction); eMANOVA 5; fMANOVA 6.

First, participants from both groups did not differ regarding baseline BMI, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 0 . 3 7 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 5 4 8 , meaning that differences between groups cannot be attributed to body weight. Second, women tended to be different on age variables, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 . 1 6 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 3 7 . Women with higher depressive symptoms tended to be younger, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 4 . 0 7 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 5 0 , to report having made their first weight-loss attempt earlier, ๐น ( 1 , 3 8 ) = 8 . 2 0 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 7 , and having started earlier to be preoccupied with their weight ๐น ( 1 , 3 8 ) = 7 . 2 5 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 1 1 , compared to women presenting lower depressive symptoms. Even though these differences were not significant with the Bonferroni correction, effect sizes were 0.67, 0.89, and 0.87, which correspond to moderate to large effects. In addition, womenโ€™ current age was significantly related to their age at first weight concerns, ๐‘Ÿ = 0 . 3 3 , โ€‰ ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 3 5 , which means that the younger women started to be preoccupied with their weight, the younger they were when they enrolled in the study. In addition, age at first weight concerns and age at first weight-loss attempt were strongly related for both groups (lower depressive symptoms group, ๐‘Ÿ = 0 . 7 3 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 1 ; higher depressive symptoms group, ๐‘Ÿ = 0 . 8 6 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 ), which means that being preoccupied earlier with body weight was related to an early weight loss attempt.

Results also showed that, following a two-month self-initiated weight-loss attempt, women with higher depressive symptoms tended to lose less weight than the other women, ๐น ( 1 , 3 4 ) = 5 . 4 1 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 2 6 . Even though the difference was not significant after the Bonferroni correction, the effect size was 0.77, which is a moderate effect. However, women with higher depressive symptoms did not differ regarding the number of previous weight-loss attempts, ๐น ( 1 , 3 8 ) = 0 . 7 9 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 3 8 0 .

Women with higher depressive symptoms also showed significantly more problematic eating behaviors, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 8 . 7 1 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 , with higher scores on disinhibition, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 1 . 1 7 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 2 , and susceptibility to hunger, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 5 . 8 4 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 . Effect sizes for eating behaviors were large, respectively, of 0.99 and 1.52. From a psychological standpoint, women with higher depressive symptoms tended to report a significantly more negative self-perception, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 4 . 2 5 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 6 , with lower scores regarding self-esteem (RSEQ), ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 6 . 8 0 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 , body esteem (BES) related to appearance, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 9 . 2 7 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 4 , and body esteem related to body weight, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 9 . 4 4 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 0 4 . Effect sizes for these differences were, respectively, of 1.24 (Self-esteem), 0.91 (Body esteem-appearance), 0.92 (Body esteem-weight), which correspond to large effects. They also tended to present lower quality of life (IWQL-Q), ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 7 . 1 1 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 1 1 . Even though this difference was not significant with the Bonferroni correction, the effect size was 0.81, which corresponds to a large effect.

Concerning personality, significant differences between groups were observed, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 6 . 0 5 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 , women with higher depressive symptoms reporting significantly lower Self-Determination, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 3 1 . 0 8 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 , and Cooperativeness, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 4 . 3 8 , ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 , on the TCI, which means that women with higher depressive symptoms reported being less confident in their power to influence their own life and less prone to accept and understand others, and to be empathic and altruistic. Effect sizes for these differences were, respectively, of 1.71 and 1.16, which correspond to large effects.

In general, results showed that there was no significant difference concerning global motivation, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 1 . 4 7 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 2 2 9 , and marginally significant differences between groups regarding motivation to regulate eating behaviors (REBS), ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 2 . 1 2 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 7 4 . Women with higher depressive symptoms tended to show more introjected motivation to regulate their eating behaviors than women with lower depressive symptoms, ๐น ( 2 , 3 0 ) = 4 . 9 8 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 3 1 , meaning that they may put on themselves some pressure to adopt particular eating behaviors in order to avoid feeling guilty and ashamed. Women with higher depressive symptoms also tended to report more amotivation, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 5 . 0 8 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 2 9 , which suggests that they may feel a lack of control over their own eating behaviors. These women also tended to report lower integrated motivation, ๐น ( 1 , 4 3 ) = 7 . 0 3 , ๐‘ƒ = 0 . 0 1 1 , meaning that they may engage in eating behaviors without having integrated the importance and value of these behaviors. Effect sizes for these differences were, respectively, of 0.68, 0.69, and 0.79, which correspond to moderate effects.

Finally, correlational analyses were computed separately for each group (Table 4). In general, in the higher depressive symptoms group, variables were more closely associated with one another than in the lower depressive symptoms group. For example, among women with higher depressive symptoms, self-esteem, body esteem related to weight, body esteem related to appearance, quality of life, and susceptibility to hunger were systematically related to depressive symptoms and correlated to one another, whereas these associations were clearly not significant among the lower depressive symptoms group. Likewise, among women with higher depressive symptoms, Self-Determination (the feeling to have an influence on oneโ€™s life) was strongly related to disinhibition, susceptibility to hunger, self-esteem, and body esteem related to weight and appearance as well as to quality of life, whereas these associations were again not significant among women who are less symptomatic. As demonstrated in Table 4, several other associations followed the same pattern.


โ€‰123456789101112

(1) Dep. symptomsโ€” โˆ’0.022 0.3700 .481 * โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— 0.642 ***0.647 ***0.184
(2) Dietary restraint โˆ’0.332 โ€” โˆ’0.046 0.0270.0610.0800.1670.1480.284 โˆ’0.014 โˆ’0.045 โˆ’0.081
(3) Disinhibition0.004โˆ’0.086โ€”0 .420 * โˆ’0.357 0 . 2 9 4 โˆ’0.407 * โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ’ โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ— โˆ— 0 . 0 5 3 0 . 1 4 3
(4) Hunger0 . 3 5 4 โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— 0 . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ— ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โ€” โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ” โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ• โˆ— โˆ’0.356 โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ– ๐Ÿ— โˆ— โˆ— 0 . 2 9 2 0 . 3 3 6
(5) Self-esteem โˆ’0.132 0.2360 . 1 8 6 0 . 1 1 6 โ€” ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ” โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ– ๐Ÿ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— 0 . ๐Ÿ• ๐Ÿ ๐ŸŽ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ‘ ๐Ÿ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ” โˆ— โˆ’0.141
(6) BE-appearance โˆ’0.088 0 . 0 4 1 โˆ’0.167 0 . 1 1 2 0 . 3 3 3 โ€”0 . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ‘ ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐Ÿ ๐Ÿ• โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— 0 . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ• ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ‘ ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โˆ’0.106
(7) BE-weight โˆ’0.062 0 . 0 2 2 โˆ’0.103 0 . 0 3 7 0 . 3 1 1 ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ– ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ’ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โ€” ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ• โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ– ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ“ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ— ๐Ÿ’ โˆ— โˆ’0.241
(8) Quality of life โˆ’0.163 0 . 1 8 0 0 . 0 5 6 0 . 1 1 8 ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ“ ๐ŸŽ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ— ๐Ÿ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โ€” ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ— ๐ŸŽ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ– ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ” โˆ— โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ‘ โˆ— โˆ’0.367
(9) Self-determination โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ” ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ– โˆ— โˆ— 0 . 2 8 3 0 . 2 6 6 โˆ’0.152 0 . 3 0 1 0 . 1 9 5 0 . 1 7 9 0 . 3 0 5 โ€” โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐ŸŽ ๐Ÿ“ โˆ— โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ— ๐Ÿ’ โˆ— โˆ— โˆ’0.387
(10) Introjected mot.0 . 2 4 8 0 . 0 9 6 0 . 0 9 0 0 . 2 1 7 โˆ’0.421 โˆ’0.374 โˆ’0.367 โˆ’0.285 โˆ’0.297 โ€”0 . ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ‘ ๐ŸŽ โˆ— โˆ— 0 . 1 9 6
(11) External mot.0.1110.1840.3400.065 โˆ’ ๐ŸŽ . ๐Ÿ’ ๐Ÿ“ ๐Ÿ“ โˆ— 0.0230.024โˆ’0.0980.2060.261โ€”0.452 *
(12) Amotivation0.068 โˆ’0.081 โˆ’0.258 0 . 0 7 4 โˆ’0.112 0 . 1 1 2 โˆ’0.050 0 . 1 6 0 โˆ’0.062 0 . 0 8 0 0 . 0 2 5 โ€”

* ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 5 , ** ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 1 , *** ๐‘ƒ < 0 . 0 0 1 . Notes. Higher depressive symptoms group above the diagonal; Lower depressive symptoms group below the diagonal.

4. Discussion

The objective of the present study was to compare two groups of overweight/obese women formed on the basis of depressive symptoms on initial weight-loss outcomes as well as on eating and psychological profiles. Several results from the present study confirmed what has been previously found [25, 26]. Women with higher depressive symptoms were more prone to lose control over eating (disinhibition), were more inclined to eat in the presence of perceptions or feelings of hunger (susceptibility to hunger), reported a lower general feeling of self-worth (self-esteem), were less satisfied with their appearance and their weight (body esteem), and presented a lower quality of life related to weight [24, 26, 27] and thus, despite the fact that they did not differ with regard to body weight compared to women with lower depressive symptoms. For women with higher depressive symptoms, other factors, besides body weight, may influence body esteem and quality of life; among this group, quality of life and body esteem related to weight were both significantly associated with depressive symptoms and disinhibition, which suggests that negative affect toward oneโ€™s self and disinhibited eating associated to a feeling of loss of control could hinder body esteem and perceived quality of life, regardless of weight.

Results also suggested that, compared to women with lower depressive symptoms, those with higher depressive symptoms were younger, have been concerned earlier by their weight (i.e., from the age of 14 for women with higher depressive symptoms and from the age of 21 for women with lower depressive symptoms), and have undertook weight-loss attempts earlier (i.e., almost seven years before, from the age of 17). In accordance, for the overall sample, current age was related to the age at first weight concerns and to the age of the first weight-loss attempt, which could explain why other studies have also found a younger age among participants from the higher depressive symptoms group. However, groups did not significantly differ regarding the number of previous weight-loss attempts, as was evidenced in previous studies [50]. This could be attributable to the small sample size and the fact that most women from both groups had undertaken many weight-loss attempts at the time of their participation in the study.

In addition, women with higher depressive symptoms tended to lose less weight than women from the other group during the two first months of a naturalistic weight-loss attempt. To our knowledge, this is the first study that examined whether subgroups separated on the basis of depressive symptoms could differ on weight-loss outcomes following a self-initiated weight-loss process. In the literature, it is suggested that depressive symptoms, even if of subclinical severity, may negatively influence weight loss [12, 28โ€“31, 51], but up until now, no study had yet documented this issue. Since it is well established that early weight loss is a significant predictor of long-term weight loss [30], we may hypothesize that women from our higher depressive symptoms group may experience more difficulties in the long-term pursuit of their weight lost. In fact, their initial failure may portend their long-term failure.

One potential explanation for this difference in terms of early weight loss is that, among women with higher depressive symptoms, all variables such as eating behaviors, self-esteem, body esteem, and quality of life were closely related to one another, whereas it was not the case among less depressed women. Results suggest that among women with higher depressive symptoms, these variables interacted to form a negative spiral, which worsens the overall picture and creates a vicious circle of distress and negative self-perception that may subsequently hinder weight-loss efforts.

In addition, women with higher depressive symptoms tended to endorse less integrated motivation, more introjected motivation, and more amotivation to regulate their eating behaviors. According to Pelletier, Dion, and colleagues [39], individuals who present these types of extrinsic motivation are more likely to endorse sociocultural pressure towards thinness and to show more problematic eating behaviors and more psychological distress than individuals presenting more intrinsic motivation types. Our results are coherent with Pelletier and colleaguesโ€™ work [39], since among women with higher depressive symptoms, the adoption of a more extrinsic motivation was related to more psychological distress, a lower self-esteem, more problematic eating behaviors (i.e., more disinhibition, more susceptibility to hunger), and a lower quality of life. Previous unsuccessful weight-loss attempts may have convinced these women that their efforts to regulate their eating behaviors had no influence on their life, or that results were depending on external forces not under their control, which would reinforce the extrinsic motivation.

Finally, it seems that Self-Determination, a personality trait, plays a central role in the psychological profile of overweight/obese women with higher depressive symptoms. Indeed, among these women, Self-Determination was linked to the majority of studied variables (depressive symptoms, disinhibition, susceptibility to hunger, self-esteem, body esteem, quality of life), whereas these links were not significant among women with lower depressive symptoms (except the link between Self-Determination and depressive symptoms). These results suggest that the incapacity to set goals for oneself and to take proper actions to achieve these goals, as well as the feeling of having no control over oneโ€™s life, is crucial among women with higher depressive symptoms. It is also interesting to interpret this result in view of results regarding motivation. Indeed, it might be that since women with higher depressive symptoms are less competent in setting goals for themselves and have less confidence in their capacity to influence their life, they rely more on external sources than on their personal resources to motivate themselves. Besides, Self-Determination was negatively linked to introjected and external motivation to regulate eating behaviors. These women who reported being less effective (Self-Determination) and presented a more extrinsic motivation also reported more negative self-esteem and body esteem, as well as a feeling of lacking control over eating (disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger), which could impede weight-loss attempts. In the end, the repetitive failures to lose weight might exacerbate the feelings of helplessness and negative self-perception that were already present. Figure 1 represents a model of the links between Self-Determination, motivation, self-perception, eating behaviors, and weight loss. Future studies will be needed to test this model in a larger sample.

However, this study also has important limitations. First, the sample size was relatively small, and after Bonferroni correction, most differences did not reach statistical significance. Yet, for the majority of these differences, effect sizes moderate or large, suggesting that substantial differences existed between groups and could be evidenced in larger samples. Additional differences could also be evidenced with a larger sample. For example, an a posteriori power analysis revealed that both groups should have been formed of at least 40 participants in order to detect significant differences concerning motivation to regulate eating behaviors. Second, ecological validity was favoured in the present study, which means that internal validity was reduced in comparison to more controlled experimental designs. Since weight-loss methods were well diversified, the efficacy of the efforts undertaken by participants could not be measured, and their adherence to recommendations was not taken into account. Finally, according to the design of the study, it is not possible to infer causal relationships between studied variables. Future studies should explore the temporal links between these variables.

In conclusion, the presence of depressive symptoms, even if they are not clinically significant, is clearly an important marker of severity regarding several eating and psychological variables among overweight/obese women. The Beck Depression Inventory seems to be useful to identify this group of women who are more vulnerable to experience difficulties during weight-loss process.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Simone Lemieux for her careful revision of the manuscript.

References

  1. World Health Organization, โ€œObesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic,โ€ Report of a WHO Consultation, Geneva, Switzerland, 2004, http://whqlibdoc.who.int/trs/WHO_TRS_894.pdf. View at: Google Scholar
  2. M. L. Fitzgibbon and D. S. Kirschenbaum, โ€œHeterogeneity of clinical presentation among obese individuals seeking treatment,โ€ Addictive Behaviors, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 291โ€“295, 1990. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  3. M. A. Friedman and K. D. Brownell, โ€œPsychological correlates of obesity: moving to the next research generation,โ€ Psychological Bulletin, vol. 117, no. 1, pp. 3โ€“20, 1995. View at: Google Scholar
  4. M. T. van der Merwe, โ€œPsychological correlates of obesity in women,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 31, supplement 2, pp. S14โ€“S18, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  5. R. A. Millstein, S. A. Carlson, J. E. Fulton et al., โ€œRelationships between body size satisfaction and weight control practices among US adults,โ€ MedGenMed Medscape General Medicine, vol. 10, no. 5, article 119, 2008. View at: Google Scholar
  6. L. A. Anderson, G. R. Janes, D. C. Ziemer, and L. S. Phillips, โ€œDiabetes in urban African Americans, body image, satisfaction with size, and weight change attempts,โ€ The Diabetes Educator, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 301โ€“308, 1997. View at: Google Scholar
  7. K. L. Green, R. Cameron, J. Polivy et al., โ€œWeight dissatisfaction and weight loss attempts among Canadian adults,โ€ Canadian Medical Association Journal, vol. 157, supplement 1, pp. S17โ€“S25, 1997. View at: Google Scholar
  8. S. R. Davy, B. A. Benes, and J. A. Driskell, โ€œSex differences in dieting trends, eating habits, and nutrition beliefs of a group of Midwestern College students,โ€ Journal of the American Dietetic Association, vol. 106, no. 10, pp. 1673โ€“1677, 2006. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  9. M. Liebman, B. A. Cameron, D. K. Carson, D. M. Brown, and S. S. Meyer, โ€œDietary fat reduction behaviors in college students: relationship to dieting status, gender and key psychosocial variables,โ€ Appetite, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 51โ€“56, 2001. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  10. A. Blokstra, C. M. Burns, and J. C. Seidell, โ€œPerception of weight status and dieting behaviour in Dutch men and women,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 7โ€“17, 1999. View at: Google Scholar
  11. B. L. Wolfe and J. E. Smith, โ€œDifferent strokes for different folks: why overweight men do not seek weight loss treatment,โ€ Eating Disorders, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 115โ€“124, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  12. K. Presnell, J. Pells, A. Stout, and G. Musante, โ€œSex differences in the relation of weight loss self-efficacy, binge eating, and depressive symptoms to weight loss success in a residential obesity treatment program,โ€ Eating Behaviors, vol. 9, no. 2, pp. 170โ€“180, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  13. J. A. Linde, R. W. Jeffery, R. L. Levy et al., โ€œBinge eating disorder, weight control self-efficacy, and depression in overweight men and women,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 28, no. 3, pp. 418โ€“425, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  14. M. Heo, A. Pietrobelli, K. R. Fontaine, J. A. Sirey, and M. S. Faith, โ€œDepressive mood and obesity in US adults: comparison and moderation by sex, age, and race,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 30, no. 3, pp. 513โ€“519, 2006. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  15. C. Dong, L. E. Sanchez, and R. A. Price, โ€œRelationship of obesity to depression: a family-based study,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 28, no. 6, pp. 790โ€“795, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  16. A. F. Jorm, A. E. Korten, H. Christensen, P. A. Jacomb, B. Rodgers, and R. A. Parslow, โ€œAssociation of obesity with anxiety, depression and emotional well-being: a community survey,โ€ Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 434โ€“440, 2003. View at: Google Scholar
  17. S. L. McElroy, R. Kotwal, S. Malhotra, E. B. Nelson, P. E. Keck Jr., and C. B. Nemeroff, โ€œAre mood disorders and obesity related? A review for the mental health professional,โ€ Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 65, no. 5, pp. 634โ€“651, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  18. R. E. Roberts, S. Deleger, W. J. Strawbridge, and G. A. Kaplan, โ€œProspective association between obesity and depression: evidence from the Alameda County study,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 514โ€“521, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  19. R. Rosmond, โ€œObesity and depression: same disease, different names?โ€ Medical Hypotheses, vol. 62, no. 6, pp. 976โ€“979, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  20. A. J. Stunkard, M. S. Faith, and K. C. Allison, โ€œDepression and obesity,โ€ Biological Psychiatry, vol. 54, no. 3, pp. 330โ€“337, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  21. F. S. Luppino, L. M. de Wit, P. F. Bouvy et al., โ€œOverweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies,โ€ Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 67, no. 3, pp. 220โ€“229, 2010. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  22. E. Atlantis and M. Baker, โ€œObesity effects on depression: systematic review of epidemiological studies,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 32, no. 6, pp. 881โ€“891, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  23. E. M. Venditti, R. R. Wing, J. M. Jakicic, B. A. Butler, and M. D. Marcus, โ€œWeight cycling, psychological health, and binge eating in obese women,โ€ Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, vol. 64, no. 2, pp. 400โ€“405, 1996. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  24. M. Q. Werrij, S. Mulkens, H. J. Hospers, and A. Jansen, โ€œOverweight and obesity: the significance of a depressed mood,โ€ Patient Education and Counseling, vol. 62, no. 1, pp. 126โ€“131, 2006. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  25. A. Jansen, R. Havermans, A. Roefs, and C. Nederkoorn, โ€œJolly fat or sad fat?: subtyping non-eating disordered overweight and obesity along an affect dimension,โ€ Appetite, vol. 51, no. 3, pp. 635โ€“640, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  26. A. Jansen, A. Vanreyten, T. van Balveren, A. Roefs, C. Nederkoorn, and R. Havermans, โ€œNegative affect and cue-induced overeating in non-eating disordered obesity,โ€ Appetite, vol. 51, no. 3, pp. 556โ€“562, 2008. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  27. M. P. Gagnon-Girouard, C. Bégin, V. Provencher, A. Tremblay, S. Boivin, and S. Lemieux, โ€œSubtyping weight-preoccupied overweight/obese women along restraint and negative affect,โ€ Appetite, vol. 55, no. 3, pp. 742โ€“745, 2010. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  28. F. Bonnet, K. Irving, J. L. Terra, P. Nony, F. Berthezène, and P. Moulin, โ€œAnxiety and depression are associated with unhealthy lifestyle in patients at risk of cardiovascular disease,โ€ Atherosclerosis, vol. 178, no. 2, pp. 339โ€“344, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  29. A. N. Fabricatore, T. A. Wadden, R. H. Moore, M. L. Butryn, S. B. Heymsfield, and A. M. Nguyen, โ€œPredictors of attrition and weight loss success: results from a randomized controlled trial,โ€ Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 47, no. 8, pp. 685โ€“691, 2009. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  30. R. A. Carels, H. M. Cacciapaglia, O. M. Douglass, S. Rydin, and W. H. O'Brien, โ€œThe early identification of poor treatment outcome in a women's weight loss program,โ€ Eating Behaviors, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 265โ€“282, 2003. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  31. S. D. Anton, C. K. Martin, L. Redman et al., โ€œPsychosocial and behavioral pre-treatment predictors of weight loss outcomes,โ€ Eating and Weight Disorders, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 30โ€“37, 2008. View at: Google Scholar
  32. E. Stice, K. Presnell, L. Groesz, and H. Shaw, โ€œEffects of a weight maintenance diet on bulimic symptoms in adolescent girls: an experimental test of the dietary restraint theory,โ€ Health Psychology, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 402โ€“412, 2005. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  33. S. Sullivan, C. R. Cloninger, T. R. Przybeck, and S. Klein, โ€œPersonality characteristics in obesity and relationship with successful weight loss,โ€ International Journal of Obesity, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 669โ€“674, 2007. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  34. Airlie Consensus Conference, Standardization of Anthropometric Measurements, Human Kinetics, Champaign, Ill, USA, 1988.
  35. A. T. Beck, R. A. Steer, and G. K. Brown, BDI-II Manuel, Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USa, 2nd edition, 1996.
  36. A. J. Stunkard and S. Messick, โ€œThe three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger,โ€ Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 29, no. 1, pp. 71โ€“83, 1985. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  37. L. G. Pelletier, C. M. Blanchard, E. Sharp, N. Otis, and C. Amiot, โ€œThe Global Motivation Scale: a dispositional measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivationfor different life activities,โ€ Manuscript submitted for publication, University of Ottawa, 2002. View at: Google Scholar
  38. E. L. Deci and R. M. Ryan, Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior, Plenum Press, New York, NY, USA, 1985.
  39. L. G. Pelletier, S. Dion, and C. Lévesque, โ€œCan self-determination help protect women against sociocultural influences about body image and reduce their risk of experiencing bulimic symptoms?โ€ Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 61โ€“88, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  40. R. L. Kolotkin, S. Head, and A. Brookhart, โ€œConstruct validity of the impact of weight on quality of life questionnaire,โ€ Obesity Research, vol. 5, no. 5, pp. 434โ€“441, 1997. View at: Google Scholar
  41. R. L. Kolotkin and R. D. Crosby, โ€œPsychometric evaluation of the impact of weight on quality of life-lite questionnaire (IWQOL-Lite) in a community sample,โ€ Quality of Life Research, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 157โ€“171, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  42. B. K. Mendelson, M. J. Mendelson, and D. R. White, โ€œBody-esteem scale for adolescents and adults,โ€ Journal of Personality Assessment, vol. 76, no. 1, pp. 90โ€“106, 2001. View at: Google Scholar
  43. M. Rosenberg, Society and Adolescent Self-Image, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965.
  44. A. Yarcheski and N. E. Mahon, โ€œA causal model of positive health practices: the relationship between approach and replication,โ€ Nursing Research, vol. 38, no. 2, pp. 88โ€“93, 1989. View at: Google Scholar
  45. T. Damji, R. Clément, and K. A. Noels, โ€œAcculturation mode, identity variation, and psychological adjustment,โ€ Journal of Social Psychology, vol. 136, no. 4, pp. 493โ€“500, 1996. View at: Google Scholar
  46. J. S. Fleming and B. E. Courtney, โ€œThe dimensionality of self-esteem: II. Hierarchical facet model for revised measurement scales,โ€ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 404โ€“421, 1984. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  47. C. R. Cloninger, The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to Its Development and Use, Washington University, Center for the Psychobiology of Personality, St. Louis, Mich, USA, 1994.
  48. C. R. Cloninger, โ€œA systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal,โ€ Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 44, no. 6, pp. 573โ€“588, 1987. View at: Google Scholar
  49. J. Cohen, Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New York, NY, USA, 2nd edition, 1988.
  50. P. Teixeira, S. B. Going, L. B. Houtkooper et al., โ€œWeight loss readiness in middle-aged women: psychosocial predictors of success for behavioral weight reduction,โ€ Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 25, no. 6, pp. 499โ€“523, 2002. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar
  51. P. J. Teixeira, A. L. Palmeira, T. L. Branco et al., โ€œWho will lose weight? A reexamination of predictors of weight loss in women,โ€ International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, vol. 1, article 12, 2004. View at: Publisher Site | Google Scholar

Copyright © 2012 C. Bégin et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.


More related articles

1322ย Views | 641ย Downloads | 3ย Citations
 PDF  Download Citation  Citation
 Download other formatsMore
 Order printed copiesOrder

Related articles

We are committed to sharing findings related to COVID-19 as quickly and safely as possible. Any author submitting a COVID-19 paper should notify us at help@hindawi.com to ensure their research is fast-tracked and made available on a preprint server as soon as possible. We will be providing unlimited waivers of publication charges for accepted articles related to COVID-19. Sign up here as a reviewer to help fast-track new submissions.